Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2009 Fall;42(3):595-608.
doi: 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-595.

Generality of the matching law as a descriptor of shot selection in basketball

Affiliations

Generality of the matching law as a descriptor of shot selection in basketball

Larry A Alferink et al. J Appl Behav Anal. 2009 Fall.

Abstract

Based on a small sample of highly successful teams, past studies suggested that shot selection (two- vs. three-point field goals) in basketball corresponds to predictions of the generalized matching law. We examined the generality of this finding by evaluating shot selection of college (Study 1) and professional (Study 3) players. The matching law accounted for the majority of variance in shot selection, with undermatching and a bias for taking three-point shots. Shot-selection matching varied systematically for players who (a) were members of successful versus unsuccessful teams, (b) competed at different levels of collegiate play, and (c) served as regulars versus substitutes (Study 2). These findings suggest that the matching law is a robust descriptor of basketball shot selection, although the mechanism that produces matching is unknown.

Keywords: basketball shot selection; choice; matching law.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Relation between shot-selection and shots-made ratios for six illustrative Division I college teams (2005–2006 season). Each data point represents one player's season-aggregate statistics. See text for details.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Results of fitting the generalized matching law to the shot-selection data of NCAA Division I teams from the 2005–2006 season. Top: variance accounted for by the matching law for 319 teams. Middle: sensitivity estimates (a parameter, or slope) for 315 teams. Bottom: bias estimates (log b parameter or y intercept) for 315 teams.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Distribution of sensitivity estimates (a parameter) derived from fitting the generalized matching law to the behavior-allocation data of individuals in laboratory choice experiments (estimated from Figure 1 of Robinson, 1992) and to the shot-selection data of 315 NCAA Division I teams. Sensitivity estimates were logarithmically transformed to normalize the distributions.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Relation between shot-selection and shots-made ratios for nine iconic professional basketball players. Each data point represents one season. In each panel, dates show the span of a player's career, with the number of seasons that were included in the analysis shown in parentheses. See text and Table 2 for details.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Baum W.M. On two types of deviation from the matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1974;22:231–242. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Baum W.M. Matching, undermatching, and overmatching in studies of choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1979;32:269–281. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Billington E.J, DiTommaso N.M. Demonstrations and applications of the matching law in education. Journal of Behavioral Education. 2003;12:91–104.
    1. Borrero J.C, Crisolo S, Tu Q, Rileand W.A, Ross N.A, Francisco M.T, et al. An application of the matching law to social dynamics. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 2007;40:589–601. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Conger R, Killeen P. Use of concurrent operants in small group research—demonstration. Pacific Sociological Review. 1974;17:399–416.

LinkOut - more resources