Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Apr;48(6):704-713.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.019.

The hierarchical structure of self-reported impulsivity

Affiliations

The hierarchical structure of self-reported impulsivity

Kris N Kirby et al. Pers Individ Dif. 2010 Apr.

Abstract

The hierarchical structure of 95 self-reported impulsivity items, along with delay-discount rates for money, was examined. A large sample of college students participated in the study (N = 407). Items represented every previously proposed dimension of self-reported impulsivity. Exploratory PCA yielded at least 7 interpretable components: Prepared/Careful, Impetuous, Divertible, Thrill and Risk Seeking, Happy-Go-Lucky, Impatiently Pleasure Seeking, and Reserved. Discount rates loaded on Impatiently Pleasure Seeking, and correlated with the impulsiveness and venturesomeness scales from the I(7) (Eysenck, Pearson, Easting, & Allsopp, 1985). The hierarchical emergence of the components was explored, and we show how this hierarchical structure may help organize conflicting dimensions found in previous analyses. Finally, we argue that the discounting model (Ainslie, 1975) provides a qualitative framework for understanding the dimensions of impulsivity.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Delay-discounting curves indicating the present values of a smaller, sooner reward available at time S (thin curve) and a larger, later reward available at time L (thick curve). At sufficiently long delays to both rewards (i.e., to the left of the crossover at C) the larger reward is preferred. Between times C and S the smaller reward is preferred, and if given an opportunity to choose during this interval this person may impulsively choose the smaller reward.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Component emergence diagram for extractions up to 8 components. The numbering notation is level-component: For example, 2-1 represents the 1st (largest) component in the 2-component extraction. Box width corresponds to the proportion of variance accounted for. Arrow labels and thicknesses show correlations larger than .30 between the component scores at adjacent levels of extraction. The number of items with loadings over .30 whose highest loading was on the component is shown in brackets. Asterisks indicate components on which discount rates (k) loaded above .30.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ainslie G. Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin. 1975;82:463–496. - PubMed
    1. Ainslie G. Breakdown of will. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY: 2001.
    1. Anderson JC, Gerbing DW. The effect of sampling error on convergence, improper solutions, and goodness-of-fit indices for maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1984;49:155–173.
    1. Audrain-McGovern J, Rodriguez D, Tercyak KP, Epstein LH, Goldman P, Wileyto EP. Applying a Behavioral Economic Framework to Understanding Adolescent Smoking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2004;18:64–73. - PubMed
    1. Avila C, Parcet MA. The role of Gray’s impulsivity in anxiety-mediated differences in resistance to extinction. European Journal of Personality. 2000;14:185–198.

LinkOut - more resources