Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2010;70(1):61-87.
doi: 10.2190/AG.70.1.c.

Does believing in "use it or lose it" relate to self-rated memory control, strategy use, and recall?

Affiliations
Comparative Study

Does believing in "use it or lose it" relate to self-rated memory control, strategy use, and recall?

Christopher Hertzog et al. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2010.

Abstract

After an oral free recall task, participants were interviewed about their memory. Despite reporting similar levels of perceived personal control over memory, older and young adults differed in the means in which they believed memory could be controlled. Older adults cited health and wellness practices and exercising memory, consistent with a "use it or lose it" belief system, more often than young adults who were more likely to mention metacognition and flexible strategy use as means of memory control. Young adults reported using more effective relational strategies during study for a free recall test. Use of relational strategies predicted recall in both age groups, but did not materially affect age differences in performance. Metacognitive beliefs, including implicit theories about aging and memory decline, memory self-concept, and perceived control over memory functioning, did not systematically correlate with strategy use or recall.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Perceived developmental changes in general memory ability for the average adult from age 20 to age 90, graphed separately for young adult and older adult raters.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Perceived developmental changes in current control over memory for the average adult from age 20 to age 90, graphed separately for young adult and older adult raters.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Perceived developmental changes in control over future memory functioning (prospective control) for the average adult from age 20 to age 90, graphed separately for young adult and older adult raters.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abelson RP. Statistics as principled argument. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 1995.
    1. Bäckman L, Larsson M. Recall of organizable words and objects in adulthood: Influences of instructions, retention interval, and retrieval cues. Journal of Gerontology. 1992;47:P273–P278. - PubMed
    1. Berry JM. Memory self-efficacy in its social cognitive context. In: Hess TM, Blanchard-Fields F, editors. Social Cognition and Aging. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 1999. pp. 69–96.
    1. Camp CJ, Markley RP, Kramer JJ. Spontaneous use of mnemonics by elderly individuals. Educational Gerontology. 1983;9:57–71.
    1. Davidson HE, Dixon RA, Hultsch DF. Memory anxiety and memory performance in adulthood. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 1991;5:423–433.

Publication types