Limits of peripheral direction discrimination of point-light walkers
- PMID: 20462316
- DOI: 10.1167/10.2.15
Limits of peripheral direction discrimination of point-light walkers
Abstract
Many previous studies have used noise tolerance to quantify sensitivity to point-light walkers heading +/-90 degrees from straight-ahead. Here we measured the smallest deviations from straight-ahead that observers could detect (azimuth thresholds) in the absence of noise. Thresholds were measured at a range of stimulus sizes and eccentricities for (1) upright and (2) inverted walkers, (3) intact walkers, those without feet and those with only feet, and (4) in the presence and absence of a second, attention-absorbing task. At large stimulus sizes azimuth thresholds were very small (between 1 and 2 degrees ) except in the case of inverted walkers. Size scaling generally compensated for eccentricity dependent sensitivity loss, however in the case of inverted walkers the data were quite noisy. At large sizes walkers without feet elicited higher thresholds than those with only feet, suggesting a special role for the feet even when walkers are not viewed side-on. Unlike others, we found no evidence that competing tasks affected performance. We argue that the value of our modified direction-discrimination task lies in its focus on the limits of discrimination within the domain of interest, rather than the amount of noise needed to impair discrimination of widely separated stimulus values.
Similar articles
-
Stimulus magnification equates identification and discrimination of biological motion across the visual field.Vision Res. 2008 Dec;48(28):2827-34. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.09.016. Epub 2008 Nov 1. Vision Res. 2008. PMID: 18848575 Clinical Trial.
-
Peripheral sensitivity to biological motion conveyed by first and second-order signals.Vision Res. 2010 Jan;50(2):127-35. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.10.020. Epub 2009 Nov 4. Vision Res. 2010. PMID: 19895827
-
Eccentric perception of biological motion is unscalably poor.Vision Res. 2005 Jul;45(15):1935-43. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.02.001. Vision Res. 2005. PMID: 15820512
-
Perceptual learning of motion direction discrimination in fovea: separable mechanisms.Vision Res. 2006 Jul;46(15):2315-27. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.01.012. Epub 2006 Mar 9. Vision Res. 2006. PMID: 16524611
-
The rate-limiting procedure of 3D DNA walkers and their applications in tandem technology.Chem Commun (Camb). 2023 Aug 24;59(69):10330-10342. doi: 10.1039/d3cc02597g. Chem Commun (Camb). 2023. PMID: 37615403 Review.
Cited by
-
Reference repulsion in the categorical perception of biological motion.Vision Res. 2012 Jul 1;64:26-34. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.05.008. Epub 2012 May 23. Vision Res. 2012. PMID: 22634421 Free PMC article.
-
Cross-modal social attention triggered by biological motion cues.J Vis. 2020 Oct 1;20(10):21. doi: 10.1167/jov.20.10.21. J Vis. 2020. PMID: 33112938 Free PMC article.
-
Sensitive perception of a person's direction of walking by 4-year-old children.Dev Psychol. 2013 Nov;49(11):2120-4. doi: 10.1037/a0031714. Epub 2013 Jan 28. Dev Psychol. 2013. PMID: 23356524 Free PMC article.
-
Figure-Ground Segmentation and Biological Motion Perception in Peripheral Visual Field.Brain Sci. 2023 Feb 22;13(3):380. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13030380. Brain Sci. 2023. PMID: 36979190 Free PMC article.
-
The application of biological motion research: biometrics, sport, and the military.Psychon Bull Rev. 2015 Feb;22(1):78-87. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0659-5. Psychon Bull Rev. 2015. PMID: 24830879 Review.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources