Familiarity breeds contempt: kangaroos persistently avoid areas with experimentally deployed dingo scents
- PMID: 20463952
- PMCID: PMC2864734
- DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010403
Familiarity breeds contempt: kangaroos persistently avoid areas with experimentally deployed dingo scents
Abstract
Background: Whether or not animals habituate to repeated exposure to predator scents may depend upon whether there are predators associated with the cues. Understanding the contexts of habituation is theoretically important and has profound implication for the application of predator-based herbivore deterrents. We repeatedly exposed a mixed mob of macropod marsupials to olfactory scents (urine, feces) from a sympatric predator (Canis lupus dingo), along with a control (water). If these predator cues were alarming, we expected that over time, some red kangaroos (Macropus rufous), western grey kangaroos (Macropus fuliginosus) and agile wallabies (Macropus agilis) would elect to not participate in cafeteria trials because the scents provided information about the riskiness of the area.
Methodology/principal findings: We evaluated the effects of urine and feces independently and expected that urine would elicit a stronger reaction because it contains a broader class of infochemicals (pheromones, kairomones). Finally, we scored non-invasive indicators (flight and alarm stomps) to determine whether fear or altered palatability was responsible for the response. Repeated exposure reduced macropodid foraging on food associated with 40 ml of dingo urine, X = 986.75+/-3.97 g food remained as compared to the tap water control, X = 209.0+/-107.0 g (P<0.001). Macropodids fled more when encountering a urine treatment, X = 4.50+/-2.08 flights, as compared to the control, X = 0 flights (P<0.001). There was no difference in effect between urine or feces treatments (P>0.5). Macropodids did not habituate to repeated exposure to predator scents, rather they avoided the entire experimental area after 10 days of trials (R(2) = 83.8; P<0.001).
Conclusions/significance: Responses to urine and feces were indistinguishable; both elicited fear-based responses and deterred foraging. Despite repeated exposure to predator-related cues in the absence of a predator, macropodids persistently avoided an area of highly palatable food. Area avoidance is consistent with that observed from other species following repeated anti-predator conditioning, However, this is the first time this response has been experimentally observed among medium or large vertebrates - where a local response is observed spatially and an area effect is revealed over time.
Conflict of interest statement
Figures



Similar articles
-
Parasite and predator risk assessment: nuanced use of olfactory cues.Proc Biol Sci. 2015 Oct 22;282(1817):20151941. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.1941. Proc Biol Sci. 2015. PMID: 26468246 Free PMC article.
-
Biologically meaningful scents: a framework for understanding predator-prey research across disciplines.Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 2018 Feb;93(1):98-114. doi: 10.1111/brv.12334. Epub 2017 Apr 26. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 2018. PMID: 28444848 Review.
-
Influence of Cat Odor on Reproductive Behavior and Physiology in the House Mouse: (Mus Musculus).In: Mucignat-Caretta C, editor. Neurobiology of Chemical Communication. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2014. Chapter 14. In: Mucignat-Caretta C, editor. Neurobiology of Chemical Communication. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2014. Chapter 14. PMID: 24830030 Free Books & Documents. Review.
-
Olfactory function in the Red Kangaroos (Macropus rufus) assessed using odor-cued taste avoidance.Physiol Behav. 1999 Sep;67(3):365-8. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00084-0. Physiol Behav. 1999. PMID: 10497954
-
Fears from the past? The innate ability of dogs to detect predator scents.Anim Cogn. 2020 Jul;23(4):721-729. doi: 10.1007/s10071-020-01379-y. Epub 2020 Apr 8. Anim Cogn. 2020. PMID: 32270350 Free PMC article.
Cited by
-
Pyrazine analogues are active components of wolf urine that induce avoidance and freezing behaviours in mice.PLoS One. 2013 Apr 24;8(4):e61753. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061753. Print 2013. PLoS One. 2013. PMID: 23637901 Free PMC article.
-
Exploiting common senses: sensory ecology meets wildlife conservation and management.Conserv Physiol. 2021 Mar 29;9(1):coab002. doi: 10.1093/conphys/coab002. eCollection 2021. Conserv Physiol. 2021. PMID: 33815799 Free PMC article.
-
Behavioral response of naïve and non-naïve deer to wolf urine.PLoS One. 2019 Nov 27;14(11):e0223248. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223248. eCollection 2019. PLoS One. 2019. PMID: 31774819 Free PMC article.
-
A review of factors influencing the stress response in Australian marsupials.Conserv Physiol. 2014 Jul 23;2(1):cou027. doi: 10.1093/conphys/cou027. eCollection 2014. Conserv Physiol. 2014. PMID: 27293648 Free PMC article.
-
Odour-mediated Interactions Between an Apex Reptilian Predator and its Mammalian Prey.J Chem Ecol. 2022 Apr;48(4):401-415. doi: 10.1007/s10886-022-01350-w. Epub 2022 Mar 1. J Chem Ecol. 2022. PMID: 35233678 Free PMC article.
References
-
- Seamans TW, Blackwell BF, Cepek JD. Coyote hair as an area repellent for white-tailed deer. Int J Pest Manage. 2002;48:301–306.
-
- Burger BV. Mammalian Semiochemicals. Top Curr Chem. 2005;240:231–278.
-
- Sankar R, Archunan G. Identification of putative pheromones in bovine (Bos taurus) faeces in relation to estrus detection. Anim Sci. 2008;103 - PubMed
-
- Gorman ML, Trowbridge BJ. The role of odour in the social lives of Carnivores. In: Gittleman J, editor. Carnivore Behavior, Ecology and Evolution. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press; 1989. pp. 57–88.
-
- Erlinge S, Sandell M, Brink C. Sexual differences in spatio-temporal interactions among badgers. Anim Behav. 1982;30:811.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Miscellaneous