Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 May 5;5(5):e10403.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010403.

Familiarity breeds contempt: kangaroos persistently avoid areas with experimentally deployed dingo scents

Affiliations

Familiarity breeds contempt: kangaroos persistently avoid areas with experimentally deployed dingo scents

Michael H Parsons et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Background: Whether or not animals habituate to repeated exposure to predator scents may depend upon whether there are predators associated with the cues. Understanding the contexts of habituation is theoretically important and has profound implication for the application of predator-based herbivore deterrents. We repeatedly exposed a mixed mob of macropod marsupials to olfactory scents (urine, feces) from a sympatric predator (Canis lupus dingo), along with a control (water). If these predator cues were alarming, we expected that over time, some red kangaroos (Macropus rufous), western grey kangaroos (Macropus fuliginosus) and agile wallabies (Macropus agilis) would elect to not participate in cafeteria trials because the scents provided information about the riskiness of the area.

Methodology/principal findings: We evaluated the effects of urine and feces independently and expected that urine would elicit a stronger reaction because it contains a broader class of infochemicals (pheromones, kairomones). Finally, we scored non-invasive indicators (flight and alarm stomps) to determine whether fear or altered palatability was responsible for the response. Repeated exposure reduced macropodid foraging on food associated with 40 ml of dingo urine, X = 986.75+/-3.97 g food remained as compared to the tap water control, X = 209.0+/-107.0 g (P<0.001). Macropodids fled more when encountering a urine treatment, X = 4.50+/-2.08 flights, as compared to the control, X = 0 flights (P<0.001). There was no difference in effect between urine or feces treatments (P>0.5). Macropodids did not habituate to repeated exposure to predator scents, rather they avoided the entire experimental area after 10 days of trials (R(2) = 83.8; P<0.001).

Conclusions/significance: Responses to urine and feces were indistinguishable; both elicited fear-based responses and deterred foraging. Despite repeated exposure to predator-related cues in the absence of a predator, macropodids persistently avoided an area of highly palatable food. Area avoidance is consistent with that observed from other species following repeated anti-predator conditioning, However, this is the first time this response has been experimentally observed among medium or large vertebrates - where a local response is observed spatially and an area effect is revealed over time.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: Dr. Michael Parsons declares a competing interest on behalf of himself, sponsor Chemistry Centre WA, Ms. Carol Lander and Mr. Warren Potts (both acknowledged in the paper). The authors have applied for an Australian patent (AU-2007202833) based on their findings.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Participation over time.
Participation by Western grey kangaroos (Macropus fuliginosus) Red kangaroos, (Macropus rufous) and Agile wallabies (Macropus agilis) attracted to trial area over a period of 11 days.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Indicators of fear.
Effects of three behavioral measures (X ± SEM) to quantify vigilance as an indicator of fear; P<0.001 for all responses except as indicated. Y-axis is average frequency of instances for each indicator: flight, stomps, feeds from treatment trough, or LN mass removed (g).
Figure 3
Figure 3. Cafeteria trials.
Western grey (Macropus fuliginosus), red kangaroos (Macropus rufous) and agile wallabies (Macropus agilis) participating in cafeteria trials in Caversham Wildlife Park, Whiteman, WA. The treatment (dingo urine) is located beside the trough in the centre. Other treatments (feces, water) were rotated among troughs and position and differed according to day.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Seamans TW, Blackwell BF, Cepek JD. Coyote hair as an area repellent for white-tailed deer. Int J Pest Manage. 2002;48:301–306.
    1. Burger BV. Mammalian Semiochemicals. Top Curr Chem. 2005;240:231–278.
    1. Sankar R, Archunan G. Identification of putative pheromones in bovine (Bos taurus) faeces in relation to estrus detection. Anim Sci. 2008;103 - PubMed
    1. Gorman ML, Trowbridge BJ. The role of odour in the social lives of Carnivores. In: Gittleman J, editor. Carnivore Behavior, Ecology and Evolution. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press; 1989. pp. 57–88.
    1. Erlinge S, Sandell M, Brink C. Sexual differences in spatio-temporal interactions among badgers. Anim Behav. 1982;30:811.

Publication types