Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 May 11;5(5):e10582.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010582.

Perception of shadows in children with autism spectrum disorders

Affiliations

Perception of shadows in children with autism spectrum disorders

Cristina Becchio et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Background: Cast shadows in visual scenes can have profound effects on visual perception. Much as they are informative, they also constitute noise as they are salient features of the visual scene potentially interfering with the processing of other features. Here we asked i) whether individuals with autism can exploit the information conveyed by cast shadows; ii) whether they are especially sensitive to noise aspects of shadows.

Methodology/principal findings: Twenty high-functioning children with autism and twenty typically developing children were asked to recognize familiar objects while the presence, position, and shape of the cast shadow were systematically manipulated. Analysis of vocal reaction time revealed that whereas typically developing children used information from cast shadows to improve object recognition, in autistic children the presence of cast shadows--either congruent or incongruent--interfered with object recognition. Critically, vocal reaction times were faster when the object was presented without a cast shadow.

Conclusions/significance: We conclude that shadow-processing mechanisms are abnormal in autism. As a result, processing shadows becomes costly and cast shadows interfere rather than help object recognition.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Examples of images used to depict the various object–shadow combinations.
The shape of the shadows could be either congruent (A) or incongruent (B) with the shape of the objects. Shadows were presented to both the right and the left of the objects. Panel C depicts an object (a bottle) without a shadow.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Graphical representation for the interaction between group (autistic, typically developing) and experimental condition (congruent, incongruent, no-shadow).
The error bars correspond to the standard errors of the means.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Mamassian P, Knill DC, Kersten D. The perception of cast shadows. Trends Cogn Sci. 1998;2:32–46. - PubMed
    1. Kersten D, Knill DC, Mamassian P, Bulthoff I. Illusory motion from shadows. Nature. 1996;379:31. - PubMed
    1. Castiello U. Implicit processing of shadows. Vision Res. 2001;41:2305–2309. - PubMed
    1. Casati R. The copycat solution to the shadow correspondence problem. Perception. 2008;37:495–503. - PubMed
    1. Mamassian P. Impossible shadows and the shadow correspondence problem. Perception. 2004;33:1279–1290. - PubMed

Publication types