Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2008 Jul 15;3(1-2):257-74.
doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0029-9.

Sensorimotor supremacy: Investigating conscious and unconscious vision by masked priming

Affiliations

Sensorimotor supremacy: Investigating conscious and unconscious vision by masked priming

Ulrich Ansorge et al. Adv Cogn Psychol. .

Abstract

According to the sensorimotor supremacy hypothesis, conscious perception draws on motor action. In the present report, we will sketch two lines of potential development in the field of masking research based on the sensorimotor supremacy hypothesis. In the first part of the report, evidence is reviewed that masked, invisible stimuli can affect motor responses, attention shifts, and semantic processes. After the review of the corresponding evidence - so-called masked priming effects - an approach based on the sensorimotor supremacy hypothesis is detailed as to how the question of a unitary mechanism of unconscious vision can be pursued by masked priming studies. In the second part of the report, different models and theories of backward masking and masked priming are reviewed. Types of models based on the sensorimotor hypothesis are discussed that can take into account ways in which sensorimotor processes (reflected in masked priming effects) can affect conscious vision under backward masking conditions.

Keywords: attention; masked priming; sensorimotor processing; vision.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Depicted is a congruent trial, with a masked target-shaped prime (e.g., a square) on the same side as the visible target shape; procedure after Klotz and Neumann (1999). Arrows stand for motion of the fixation dots (toward the screen center). For details refer to the text.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Depicted is an invalid trial, with a masked shape-singleton prime in the upper left corner (the one red diamond among the three different, shape-homogenous square primes) in the first display (depicted in the background) followed by square-shaped masks at all four positions (depicted in the foreground), with one of the masking squares serving as a target. In the depicted example trial, participants have to search for a black target square (in the upper right corner of the depicted display), and have to respond to its location (i.e., whether it is left or right). Thus, the trial is invalid because the masked singleton prime is presented at a position away from the target. Note that the masked shape-singleton prime is task-irrelevant in almost every respect. It has a color and a shape different from that of the target. Thus, participants have no reason to intentionally search for the shape or for the color of the shape-singleton prime. Furthermore, under the depicted conditions, participants have also no incentive to search for a singleton by intention, because the target is not a singleton either (neither with respect to its shape nor its color). Would the masked shape-singleton prime still capture attention away from the target? (Under the depicted conditions this prediction is made by theories assuming that attention is captured to locations containing the largest feature differences relative to the rest of the display.) The corresponding attentional effect would be reflected in posterior ERP laterality indices (compared to conditions with masked shape-singleton prime and target being presented on the same side, e.g., both being on the right). Note that under the depicted conditions, stimulus intensity in the priming display is the same at all positions. Therefore, any index of attentional capture by the masked shape-singleton prime cannot be attributed to stimulus intensity. (Arrow: direction of time.)

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Allport A. Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action. In: Heuer H., Sanders A. F., editors. Perspectives on perception and action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1987. pp. 395–419.
    1. Ansorge U. Asymmetric influences of temporally vs. nasally presented masked visual information: Evidence for collicular contributions to nonconscious priming effects. Brain and Cognition. 2003;51:317–325. - PubMed
    1. Ansorge U. Top-down contingencies of nonconscious priming revealed by dual-task interference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2004;57A:1123–1148. - PubMed
    1. Ansorge U., Heumann M. Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2003;29:937–948. - PubMed
    1. Ansorge U., Heumann M. Peripheral cuing by abrupt-onset cues: The role of color in S-R corresponding conditions. Acta Psychologica. 2004;116:115–143. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources