Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Controlled Clinical Trial
. 2011 Jan;19(1):69-73.
doi: 10.1038/oby.2010.138. Epub 2010 Jun 10.

Cognitive interference from food cues in weight loss maintainers, normal weight, and obese individuals

Affiliations
Free PMC article
Controlled Clinical Trial

Cognitive interference from food cues in weight loss maintainers, normal weight, and obese individuals

Suzanne Phelan et al. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2011 Jan.
Free PMC article

Abstract

Much attention has been paid to the behavioral characteristics of successful weight loss maintenance, but less is known about the cognitive processes that underlie this process. The purpose of this study was to investigate cognitive interference from food-related cues in long-term weight loss maintainers (WLM; N = 15) as compared with normal weight (NW; N = 19) and obese (OB; N = 14) controls. A Food Stroop paradigm was used to determine whether successful WLM differed from controls in both the speed and accuracy of color naming words for low-calorie and high-calorie foods. A significant group × condition interaction for reaction time was observed (P = 0.04). In post hoc analyses, no significant differences in reaction time across the three groups were observed for the low-calorie foods (P = 0.66). However, for the high-calorie foods, WLM showed a significantly slower reaction time than the NW (0.04) and OB (0.009) groups (885 ± 17.6, 834 ± 15.8, 816 ± 18.3 ms, respectively). No significant group differences were seen for number of correct trials in 45 s (P = 0.12). The differential interference among WLM did not appear to generalize to other types of distracters (i.e., nonfood). Overall, findings from this study suggest that WLM differ from OB and NW controls in their cognitive responses to high-calorie food cues. Future research is needed to better understand why this bias exists and whether and how interventions can change cognitive processes to better facilitate long-term weight control.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Reaction time among weight loss maintainer (n = 15), normal weight (n = 19), and obese (n =14) groups during the Food Stroop. NW, normal weight; OB, obese; WLM, weight loss maintainers.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Phelan S, Lang W, Jordan D, Wing RR. Use of artificial sweeteners and fat-modified foods in weight loss maintainers and always-normal weight individuals. Int J Obes (Lond) 2009;33:1183–1190. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Phelan S, Roberts M, Lang W, Wing RR. Empirical evaluation of physical activity recommendations for weight control in women. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39:1832–1836. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Klem ML, Wing RR, McGuire MT, Seagle HM, Hill JO. A descriptive study of individuals successful at long-term maintenance of substantial weight loss. Am J Clin Nutr. 1997;66:239–246. - PubMed
    1. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Kempen KP, Saris WH. Determinants of weight maintenance in women after diet-induced weight reduction. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1998;22:1–6. - PubMed
    1. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Experimenl Psychol. 1935;18:643–662.

Publication types