Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Jun;25(2):356-68.
doi: 10.1037/a0017625.

Age-related decline of visual processing components in change detection

Affiliations

Age-related decline of visual processing components in change detection

Matthew C Costello et al. Psychol Aging. 2010 Jun.

Abstract

Previous research has suggested that an age-related decline in change detection may be due to older adults using a more conservative response criterion. However, this finding may reflect methodological limitations of the traditional change detection design, in which displays are presented continuously until a change is detected. Across 2 experiments, the authors assessed adult age differences in a version of change detection that required a response after each pair of pre- and postchange displays, thus reducing the potential contribution of response criterion. Older adults performed worse than younger adults, committing more errors and requiring a greater number of display cycles for correct detection. These age-related performance declines were substantially reduced after controlling statistically for elementary perceptual speed. Search strategy was largely similar for the 2 age groups, but perceptual speed was less successful in accounting for age-related variance in detectability when a more precise spatial localization of change was required (Experiment 2). Thus, the negative effect of aging in the present tasks lies in a reduction of detection efficiency due largely to processing speed, though some strategy-level effects may also contribute. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved).

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Stimuli and course of events in a typical trial of Experiment 1. After each display of the image-pairs, participants gave their detection response: didn't see, possibly saw but needing to verify, and saw. The image-pairs were repeatedly shown when participants indicated a negative detection response. When participants responded with positive detection, they were prompted to indicate whether the changed item was located on the left or right side of the display.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Hits minus false alarms (A) and cycles to detection (B) measures of Experiment 1, as a function of age group. Error bars represent standard errors.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Stimuli and course of events in a typical trial of Experiment 2. After each display of the image-pairs, participants used the mouse to click on the screen indicating where they thought the changed item was located. After each localization response, participants indicated a detection response: didn't see, possibly saw but needing to verify, and saw. The image-pairs were repeatedly shown until either correct or failed detection.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Hits minus false alarms (A) and cycles to detection (B) measures of Experiment 2, as a function of age group. Error bars represent standard errors.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Illustration of the temporal integration and focused attention models, with hypothetical localization error for change trials leading to correct detection, as a function of model type and cycles prior to detection. Cycle -1 represents the display-pair presented immediately prior to correct detection (Cycle 0, not displayed). Older adult performance will approximate the temporal integration model if they perform change detection with a more conservative response criterion compared to younger adults.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Mean localization error (in cm) for change trials with successful detection of the changed item, as a function of age group and cycles prior to detection.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Angelone BL, Levin DT, Simons DJ. The roles of representation and comparison failures in change blindness. Perception. 2003;32:947. - PubMed
    1. Bach M. The Freiburg Visual Acuity test--automatic measurement of visual acuity. Optometry Vision Science. 1996;73:49–53. - PubMed
    1. Ball KK, Roenker DL, Bruni JR. Developmental changes in attention and visual search throughout adulthood. In: Enns JT, editor. The development of attention: Research and theory. Amsterdam: North Holland; 1990. pp. 489–508.
    1. Beck MR, Levin DT. The role of representational volatility in recognizing pre- and post-change objects. Perception & Psychophysics. 2003;65:458–468. - PubMed
    1. Caird JK, Edwards CJ, Creaser JI, Horrey WJ. Older driver failures of attention at intersections: Using change blindness methods to assess turn decision accuracy. Human Factors. 2005;47:235–249. - PubMed

Publication types