Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2011 Jan;16(1):152-62.
doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00228.x.

Nicotine competes with a visual stimulus for control of conditioned responding

Affiliations

Nicotine competes with a visual stimulus for control of conditioned responding

Jennifer E Murray et al. Addict Biol. 2011 Jan.

Abstract

Environmental stimuli that co-occur with tobacco use come to evoke drug-related conditioned responses (CRs) that appear involved in continued use of nicotine-containing products. In rats, nicotine can serve as a conditional stimulus (CS) for non-drug unconditioned stimuli (USs), prompting the question of whether the nicotine CS can compete with, or overshadow, a non-drug environmental stimulus for control of a CR. In Experiment 1, male Sprague-Dawley rats were assigned to a group [0, 0.01, 0.03, 0.045, or 0.06 mg nicotine (base)/kg/infusion]. During each session, there were 10 intravenous infusions followed by a 30-second houselight to form a compound CS. At light offset there was 4-second access to sucrose. For Experiment 2, groups were nicotine (0.03 mg/kg/infusion) + light compound paired, nicotine + light compound unpaired, nicotine paired and light unpaired, and nicotine unpaired and light paired. Paired stimuli were presented with sucrose similar to Experiment 1. Unpaired stimuli were temporally separated from sucrose. Following acquisition, tests of nicotine and light alone were conducted by intermixing non-reinforced trails into training sessions. Nicotine dose-dependently overshadowed the light CS as shown by reduced light control of conditioned responding with higher doses. The nicotine, light, and nicotine + light compound had to be paired with sucrose to evoke a CR. These results demonstrate nicotine overshadows an exteroceptive visual stimulus. Because exteroceptive stimuli are often the focus of cue-exposure therapy, such competition may help begin to explain the marginal effectiveness of these therapies.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Acquisition and maintenance of compound stimulus training. Mean conditioned responding [±1 standard error of the mean (SEM)] for each training session in all groups is shown (a). Conditioned responding in acquisition and maintenance phases of compound training with results of the element test sessions (+1 SEM) are shown for the individual training groups (b–f ).The order of element test presentation is for display purposes only; they were conducted in a counterbalanced manner (see Procedures). Significant results are described in the text
Figure 2
Figure 2
Tests 1 and 2 (a and b, respectively) of the proportion of total elevation scores (+1 SEM) for responding on the nicotine and light test trials for each of the five groups are shown. * denotes significant difference between proportion of responding on the nicotine and light elements within the group. + denotes a significantly higher proportion of nicotine-evoked responding compared with the 0 mg/kg group. # denotes a significantly lower proportion of light-evoked responding compared with the 0 mg/kg group
Figure 3
Figure 3
Acquisition and maintenance of stimulus training. Mean elevation scores (±1 standard error of the mean) of acquisition and element tests for the four training groups in Experiment 4 (a, b, c and d display the N−/L+, N+/L−, NL+, and NL− groups, respectively) are shown. The order of element test presentation is for display purposes only; they were conducted in a counterbalanced manner (see Procedures). Significant effects are described in the text
Figure 4
Figure 4
Tests 1 and 2 (a and b, respectively) of elevation scores (+1 standard error of the mean) for the nicotine and light test trials for each of the four groups are shown. * denotes significant difference between responding on the nicotine and light elements within the group. + denotes significantly lower nicotine-evoked responding compared with the N+/L− group. # denotes significantly lower light-evoked responding compared with the N−/L+ group

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alessi SM, Roll JM, Reilly MP, Johanson C-E. Establishment of a diazepam preference in human volunteers following differential-conditioning history of placebo versus diazepam choice. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2002;10:77–83. - PubMed
    1. Becker KM, Rose JE, Albino AP. A randomized trial of nicotine replacement therapy in combination with reduced-nicotine replacement therapy in combination with reduced-nicotine cigarettes for smoking cessation. Nicotine Tob Res. 2008;10:1139–1148. - PubMed
    1. Besheer J, Palmatier MI, Metschke DM, Bevins RA. Nicotine as a signal for the presence or absence of sucrose reward: a Pavlovian drug appetitive conditioning preparation in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2004;172:108–117. - PubMed
    1. Bevins RA. The reference-dose place conditioning procedure yields a graded dose-effect function. Int J Comp Psychol. 2005;18:101–111.
    1. Bevins RA. Altering the motivational function of nicotine through conditioning processes. In: Bevins RA, Caggiula AR, editors. The Motivational Impact of Nicotine and Its Role in Tobacco Use: The 55th Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. New York: Springer; 2009. pp. 111–129. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types