Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2011 Jan 22;278(1703):211-7.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1142. Epub 2010 Jul 28.

Automatic imitation in dogs

Affiliations

Automatic imitation in dogs

Friederike Range et al. Proc Biol Sci. .

Abstract

After preliminary training to open a sliding door using their head and their paw, dogs were given a discrimination task in which they were rewarded with food for opening the door using the same method (head or paw) as demonstrated by their owner (compatible group), or for opening the door using the alternative method (incompatible group). The incompatible group, which had to counterimitate to receive food reward, required more trials to reach a fixed criterion of discrimination performance (85% correct) than the compatible group. This suggests that, like humans, dogs are subject to 'automatic imitation'; they cannot inhibit online the tendency to imitate head use and/or paw use. In a subsequent transfer test, where all dogs were required to imitate their owners' head and paw use for food reward, the incompatible group made a greater proportion of incorrect, counterimitative responses than the compatible group. These results are consistent with the associative sequence learning model, which suggests that the development of imitation depends on sensorimotor experience and phylogenetically general mechanisms of associative learning. More specifically, they suggest that the imitative behaviour of dogs is shaped more by their developmental interactions with humans than by their evolutionary history of domestication.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Diagrams of (a) the training box, and (b) the test box and its door.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Photographs of (a) a dog owner demonstrating how to open the box with the hand (paw); (b) dog matching the paw action; (c) dog owner demonstrating how to open the box with the head; and (d) dog matching the head action.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
The mean (+ s.e.m.) number of trials to reach criterion for dogs in the compatible and incompatible groups in the training phase when no commands were given (phase 1).
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
The mean (+s.e.m.) discrimination ratio for dogs in the compatible and incompatible groups in the transfer test (phase 2).

References

    1. Sturmer B., Aschersleben G., Prinz W. 2000. Effects of correspondence between complex stimulus and response patterns. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26, 1746–175910.1037/0096-1523.26.6.1746 (doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.6.1746) - DOI - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brass M., Bekkering H., Prinz W. 2001. Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychol. 106, 3–2210.1016/S0001-6918(00)00024-X (doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(00)00024-X) - DOI - DOI - PubMed
    1. Kilner J. M., Paulignan Y., Blakemore S. J. 2003. An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Curr. Biol. 13, 522–52510.1016/S0960-9822(03)00165-9 (doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00165-9) - DOI - DOI - PubMed
    1. Heyes C. M., Bird G., Johnson H., Haggard P. 2005. Experience modulates automatic imitation. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 233–24010.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.009 (doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.009) - DOI - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bach P., Tipper S. P. 2007. Implicit action encoding influences personal-trait judgments. Cognition 102, 151–17810.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.003 (doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.003) - DOI - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources