Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Sep 1;13(5):738-51.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00935.x.

Medial temporal lobe memory in childhood: developmental transitions

Affiliations

Medial temporal lobe memory in childhood: developmental transitions

Elise L Townsend et al. Dev Sci. .

Abstract

The medial temporal lobes (MTL) support declarative memory and mature structurally and functionally during the postnatal years in humans. Although recent work has addressed the development of declarative memory in early childhood, less is known about continued development beyond this period of time. The purpose of this investigation was to explore MTL-dependent memory across middle childhood. Children (6 -10 years old) and adults completed two computerized tasks, place learning (PL) and transitive inference (TI), that each examined relational memory, as well as the flexible use of relational learning. Findings suggest that the development of relational memory precedes the development of the ability to use relational knowledge flexibly in novel situations. Implications for the development of underlying brain areas and ideas for future neuroimaging investigations are discussed.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Schematic illustrating the five oval stimuli for the TI task
Figure 2
Figure 2
Premise pairs for the TI task
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean latency to locate the hidden target (seconds ±1SE) for 6, 8, 10- year olds and adults in the place learning (PL) task. Between 6 and 10 years, there is a gradual, linear decline in latency to relocate a hidden target using visual cues (relational memory). In contrast, latency to a hidden target with a subset of cues removed (flexible memory use) shows a significant shift between 8 and 10 years, with 6- and 8-year olds performing similarly and 10- year olds performing like adults.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Mean accuracy (±1SE) for premise and BD inference pairs in the transitive inference (TI) task. Between 6 and 10 years, there is gradual improvement in premise pair accuracy (relational memory). In contrast, BD inference pair accuracy (flexible memory use) shows a significant shift between 8 and 10 years, with 6 and 8-year olds performing similarly and 10-year olds performing like adults.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Acredolo L. Development of spatial orientation in infancy. Developmental Psychology. 1978;14(3):224–234.
    1. Acuna B, Eliassen J, Donoghue JP, Sanes J. Frontal and parietal lobe activation during transitive inference in humans. Cerebral Cortex. 2002;12:1312–1321. - PubMed
    1. Alvarado M, Bachevalier J. Revisiting the maturation of medial temporal lobe memory functions in primates. Learning and Memory. 2000;7:244–256. - PubMed
    1. Astur RS, Constable RT. Hippocampus Activation during Virtual Tasks: Comparing the Morris Water Task with the 8-Arm Maze during fMRI; Paper presented at the Cognitive Neuroscience Society; San Francisco. 2002.
    1. Astur RS, Taylor LB, Mamelak AN, Philpott L, Sutherland RJ. Humans with hippocampus damage display severe spatial memory impairments in a virtual Morris water task. Behavioral Brain Research. 2002;132(1):77–84. - PubMed

Publication types