Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2011 Jan 1;64(1):18-31.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.09.004.

False Memories for Suggestions: The Impact of Conceptual Elaboration

Affiliations

False Memories for Suggestions: The Impact of Conceptual Elaboration

Maria S Zaragoza et al. J Mem Lang. .

Abstract

Relatively little attention has been paid to the potential role that reflecting on the meaning and implications of suggested events (i.e., conceptual elaboration) might play in promoting the creation of false memories. Two experiments assessed whether encouraging repeated conceptual elaboration, would, like perceptual elaboration, increase false memory for suggested events. Results showed that conceptual elaboration of suggested events more often resulted in high confidence false memories (Experiment 1) and false memories that were accompanied by the phenomenal experience of remembering them (Experiment 2) than did surface-level processing. Moreover, conceptual elaboration consistently led to higher rates of false memory than did perceptual elaboration. The false memory effects that resulted from conceptual elaboration were highly dependent on the organization of the postevent interview questions, such that conceptual elaboration only increased false memory beyond surface level processing when participants evaluated both true and suggested information in relation to the same theme or dimension.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean proportion of suggested items misattributed to the video (collapsed across maybe yes, probably yes, and definitely yes responses) as a function of postevent exposure (0 vs. 3) and elaboration condition (No Elaboration, Perceptual, or Conceptual). Note that the pattern is identical for the definitely yes errors. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean proportion of items seen in the video that were correctly attributed to the video (collapsed across maybe yes, probably yes, and definitely yes) as a function of postevent exposure (0 vs. 3) and elaboration condition (No Elaboration, Perceptual, or Conceptual). The pattern is the same for definitely yes responses. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean proportion of suggested items in the 3 postevent exposure condition that were incorrectly remembered as being in the video, as a function of elaboration condition (No Elaboration, Perceptual, or Conceptual) and grouping condition (By Episode or By Scene). Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Bergman E, Roediger HL. Can Bartlett’s repeated reproduction experiments be replicated? Memory & Cognition. 1999;27:937–947. - PubMed
    1. Bransford JD, Johnson MK. Considerations of some problems of comprehension. In: Chase W, editor. Visual information processing. Academic Press; New York: 1973. pp. 383–438.
    1. Brewer WF, Treyens JC. Role of schemata in memory for places. Cognitive Psychology. 1981;13:207–230.
    1. Ceci SJ, Crotteau Huffman ML, Smith E, Loftus EF. Repeatedly thinking about non-events. Consciousness and Cognition. 1994;3:388–407.
    1. Chrobak Q, Zaragoza MS. Inventing stories: Forcing witnesses to fabricate entire fictitious events leads to freely reported false memories. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 2008;15(6):1190–1195. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources