Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2011 Feb;11(1):90-100.
doi: 10.1037/a0021551.

The malleability of age-related positive gaze preferences: training to change gaze and mood

Affiliations

The malleability of age-related positive gaze preferences: training to change gaze and mood

Derek M Isaacowitz et al. Emotion. 2011 Feb.

Abstract

Older adults show positive gaze preferences, but to what extent are these preferences malleable? Examining the plasticity of age-related gaze preferences may provide a window into their origins. We therefore designed an attentional training procedure to assess the degree to which we could shift gaze and gaze-related mood in both younger and older adults. Participants completed either a positive or negative dot-probe training. Before and after the attentional training, we obtained measures of fixations to negatively valenced images along with concurrent mood ratings. We found differential malleability of gaze and mood by age: for young adults, negative training resulted in fewer posttraining fixations to the most negative areas of the images, whereas positive training appeared more successful in changing older adults' fixation patterns. Young adults did not differ in their moods as a function of training, whereas older adults in the train negative group had the worst moods after training. Implications for the etiology of age-related positive gaze preferences are considered.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Fixations to the most negative areas of the IAPS images from pre-training to post-training, for each training condition and age group.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Change in percent fixation to the negative areas of the IAPS images from pre-training to post-training, for each training condition and age group, such that negative values indicate decreased fixation in post-training compared to pre-training. Error bars depict standard errors of the means.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Change in percent fixation to the entire IAPS images from pre-training to post-training, for each training condition and age group, such that negative values indicate decreased fixation in post-training compared to pre-training. Similarly, positive values indicate increased fixation in post-training compared to pre-training. Error bars depict standard errors of the means.

References

    1. Allard ES, Isaacowitz DM. Are preferences in emotional processing affected by distraction? Examining the age-related positivity effect in visual fixation within a dual-task paradigm. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 2008;15:725–743. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Baltes P. Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology. 1987;23:611–626.
    1. Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Finkenauer C, Vohs KD. Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology. 2001;5:323–370.
    1. Birditt KS, Fingerman KL, Almeida DM. Age differences in exposure and reactions to interpersonal tensions: A daily diary study. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20:330–340. - PubMed
    1. Blanchard-Fields F. Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:26–31.

Publication types