Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Meta-Analysis
. 2011 May 10;108(19):7716-20.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018601108. Epub 2011 Apr 25.

Role of test motivation in intelligence testing

Affiliations
Meta-Analysis

Role of test motivation in intelligence testing

Angela Lee Duckworth et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Retraction in

Expression of concern in

Abstract

Intelligence tests are widely assumed to measure maximal intellectual performance, and predictive associations between intelligence quotient (IQ) scores and later-life outcomes are typically interpreted as unbiased estimates of the effect of intellectual ability on academic, professional, and social life outcomes. The current investigation critically examines these assumptions and finds evidence against both. First, we examined whether motivation is less than maximal on intelligence tests administered in the context of low-stakes research situations. Specifically, we completed a meta-analysis of random-assignment experiments testing the effects of material incentives on intelligence-test performance on a collective 2,008 participants. Incentives increased IQ scores by an average of 0.64 SD, with larger effects for individuals with lower baseline IQ scores. Second, we tested whether individual differences in motivation during IQ testing can spuriously inflate the predictive validity of intelligence for life outcomes. Trained observers rated test motivation among 251 adolescent boys completing intelligence tests using a 15-min "thin-slice" video sample. IQ score predicted life outcomes, including academic performance in adolescence and criminal convictions, employment, and years of education in early adulthood. After adjusting for the influence of test motivation, however, the predictive validity of intelligence for life outcomes was significantly diminished, particularly for nonacademic outcomes. Collectively, our findings suggest that, under low-stakes research conditions, some individuals try harder than others, and, in this context, test motivation can act as a third-variable confound that inflates estimates of the predictive validity of intelligence for life outcomes.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Hypothesized associations among IQ, test motivation, life outcomes, and latent variables.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Model tested in Study 2. Values are standardized regression coefficients, and parenthetical values are from the IQ-only model, which did not include test motivation or its associations with IQ scores and outcomes. The thickness of regression paths are proportional to standardized coefficients. All paths are significantly different from zero; P < 0.05. Demographic covariates and covariances among life outcomes are not shown.

References

    1. Gottfredson LS. Intelligence: Is it the epidemiologists’ elusive “fundamental cause” of social class inequalities in health? J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;86:174–199. - PubMed
    1. Hogan R. In defense of personality measurement: New wine for old whiners. Hum Perform. 2005;18:331–341.
    1. Jensen AR. The G Factor: The Science of Mental Ability. Westport, CT: Praeger/ Greenwood; 1998.
    1. Judge TA, Colbert AE, Ilies R. Intelligence and leadership: A quantitative review and test of theoretical propositions. J Appl Psychol. 2004;89:542–552. - PubMed
    1. Neisser U, et al. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. Am Psychol. 1996;51:77–101.

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources