Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Clinical Trial
. 2011 Jun 14;108(24):9998-10003.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1102433108. Epub 2011 May 31.

Unraveling the distributed neural code of facial identity through spatiotemporal pattern analysis

Affiliations
Clinical Trial

Unraveling the distributed neural code of facial identity through spatiotemporal pattern analysis

Adrian Nestor et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

Face individuation is one of the most impressive achievements of our visual system, and yet uncovering the neural mechanisms subserving this feat appears to elude traditional approaches to functional brain data analysis. The present study investigates the neural code of facial identity perception with the aim of ascertaining its distributed nature and informational basis. To this end, we use a sequence of multivariate pattern analyses applied to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data. First, we combine information-based brain mapping and dynamic discrimination analysis to locate spatiotemporal patterns that support face classification at the individual level. This analysis reveals a network of fusiform and anterior temporal areas that carry information about facial identity and provides evidence that the fusiform face area responds with distinct patterns of activation to different face identities. Second, we assess the information structure of the network using recursive feature elimination. We find that diagnostic information is distributed evenly among anterior regions of the mapped network and that a right anterior region of the fusiform gyrus plays a central role within the information network mediating face individuation. These findings serve to map out and characterize a cortical system responsible for individuation. More generally, in the context of functionally defined networks, they provide an account of distributed processing grounded in information-based architectures.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Experimental face stimuli (4 identities × 4 expressions). Stimuli were matched with respect to low-level properties (e.g., mean luminance), external features (hair), and high-level characteristics (e.g., sex). Face images courtesy of the Face-Place Face Database Project (http://www.face-place.org/) Copyright 2008, Michael J. Tarr. Funding provided by NSF Award 0339122.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Group information-based map of face individuation. The map is computed using a searchlight (SL) approach and estimates the discriminability of facial identities across expression (q < 0.05). Each voxel in the map represents the center of an SL-defined region supporting identity discrimination. The four slices show the sensitivity peaks of the four clusters revealed by this analysis.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Sensitivity estimates in three ROIs. Facial identity discrimination was computed using both the entire set of features in an ROI and a subset of diagnostic features identified by multivariate feature selection (i.e., RFE), the two types of classification are labeled as pre- and post-RFE. The average number of features involved in classification is superimposed on each bar. The results indicate that the bilateral FFA, in contrast to an early visual area, contains sufficient information to discriminate identities above chance (P < 0.05).
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
Spatiotemporal distribution of information diagnostic for face individuation. (A) Group map of average feature ranking for the top 400 features—rows show different slices and columns different time points. Color codes the ranking of the features across space (the four regions identified by our SL analysis) and time (4–8 s poststimulus onset). The map shows a lower concentration of features in the lpFG relative to other regions but a comparable number of features across time. (B) Average feature distribution across subjects by cluster and time point (the bar graph quantifies the results illustrated in A). (C) Time course of feature elimination by ROI for 4,000 features (top 1,000 features for each ROI). This analysis confirms that lpFG features are eliminated at a higher rate, indicative of their reduced diagnosticity (shaded areas show ±1 SE across subjects).
Fig. 5.
Fig. 5.
Pairwise ROI relations. The pattern of identity (mis)classifications is separately compared for each pair of regions using correlation-based scores (red) and mutual information (brown). Specifically, we relate classification results across regions while controlling for the pattern of true labels. These measures are used as a proxy for assessing similarity in the encoding of facial identity across regions. Of the four ROIs, the raFG produced the highest scores in its relationship with the other regions (connector width is proportional to z values).

References

    1. DiCarlo JJ, Cox DD. Untangling invariant object recognition. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:333–341. - PubMed
    1. Jiang X, et al. Evaluation of a shape-based model of human face discrimination using FMRI and behavioral techniques. Neuron. 2006;50:159–172. - PubMed
    1. Kanwisher N. Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:11163–11170. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Fairhall SL, Ishai A. Effective connectivity within the distributed cortical network for face perception. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2400–2406. - PubMed
    1. Gauthier I, et al. The fusiform “face area” is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12:495–504. - PubMed

Publication types