Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2011 Mar 18:2:43.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00043. eCollection 2011.

Novelty vs. Familiarity Principles in Preference Decisions: Task-Context of Past Experience Matters

Affiliations

Novelty vs. Familiarity Principles in Preference Decisions: Task-Context of Past Experience Matters

Hsin-I Liao et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Our preferences are shaped by past experience in many ways, but a systematic understanding of the factors is yet to be achieved. For example, studies of the mere exposure effect show that experience with an item leads to increased liking (familiarity preference), but the exact opposite tendency is found in other studies utilizing dishabituation (novelty preference). Recently, it has been found that image category affects whether familiarity or novelty preference emerges from repeated stimulus exposure (Park et al., 2010). Faces elicited familiarity preference, but natural scenes elicited novelty preference. In their task, preference judgments were made throughout all exposures, raising the question of whether the task-context during exposure was involved. We adapt their paradigm, testing if passive exposure or objective judgment task-contexts lead to different results. Results showed that after passive viewing, familiar faces were preferred, but no preference bias in either direction was found with natural scenes, or with geometric figures (control). After exposure during the objective judgment task, familiar faces were preferred, novel natural scenes were preferred, and no preference bias was found with geometric figures. The overall results replicate the segregation of preference biases across object categories and suggest that the preference for familiar faces and novel natural scenes are modulated by task-context memory at different processing levels or selection involvement. Possible underlying mechanisms of the two types of preferences are discussed.

Keywords: judgment; memory; perception; preference; social cognition.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Stimuli and procedure. (A) Sample judgment phase trial with natural scene images. (B) Examples of face and geometric figure images.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean preference bias among initial preference judgments in Experiments 1 and 2. Error bars represent one standard error of the mean. Asterisks indicate statistically significant differences from 0.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Timelines of preference bias in Experiment 1. Data points are trial-by-trial means of ratings rectified toward the repeated image. Error bars represent one standard error of the mean.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Timelines of rating bias in Experiment 2. Data points are trial-by-trial means of ratings rectified toward the repeated image. Error bars represent one standard error of the mean.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adolphs R., Tranel D., Koenigs M., Damasio A. R. (2005). Preferring one taste over another without recognizing either. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 860–861 - PubMed
    1. Berlyne D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Percept. Psychophys. 8, 279–286
    1. Bevins R. A., Bardo M. T. (1999). Conditioned increase in place preference by access to novel objects: antagonism by MK-801. Behav. Brain Res. 99, 53–60 - PubMed
    1. Bornstein R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychol. Bull. 106, 265–289
    1. Bornstein R. F., D'Agostino P. R. (1992). Stimulus recognition and the mere exposure effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 545–552 - PubMed