Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2000 May;42(4):481-496.
doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2692.

Perception of Wordlikeness: Effects of Segment Probability and Length on the Processing of Nonwords

Affiliations

Perception of Wordlikeness: Effects of Segment Probability and Length on the Processing of Nonwords

Stefan A Frisch et al. J Mem Lang. 2000 May.

Abstract

A probabilistic phonotactic grammar based on the probabilities of the constituents contained in a dictionary of English was used to generate multisyllabic nonwords. English-speaking listeners evaluated the wordlikeness of these patterns. Wordlikeness ratings were higher for nonwords containing high-probability constituents and were also higher for nonwords with fewer syllables. Differences in the processing of these same nonwords that partially reflected their perceived wordlikeness were also found in a recognition memory task. Nonwords with higher probability constituents yielded better recognition memory performance, suggesting that participants were able to use their knowledge of frequently occurring lexical patterns to improve recognition. These results suggest that lexical patterns provide the foundation of an emergent phonological competence used to process nonwords in both linguistic and metalinguistic tasks.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIG. 1
FIG. 1
Structured phonological representation of example nonwords created from the eight constituent distributions, one for each length. The two- and four-syllable nonwords have high constituent probability and the three-syllable nonword has low constituent probability. σs, strong syllable (stressed); σw, weak syllable (unstressed); O, onset; R, rime.
FIG. 2
FIG. 2
Mean subjective ratings for the nonword stimuli in Experiment 1 for each constituent probability group (High vs Low) and length in syllables (two, three, or four). Error bars show 99% confidence interval for the mean.
FIG. 3
FIG. 3
Mean subjective ratings for each nonword in Experiment 1 as a function of the log product of onset and rime probabilities for the nonword.
FIG. 4
FIG. 4
Percentage of participants responding with a rating higher than an implicit rejection for each nonword in Experiment 1 as a function of the log product probability.
FIG. 5
FIG. 5
Percentage of acceptances for each nonword in Experiment 2 for each constituent probability group (High vs Low) and length in syllables (two, three, or four). Error bars show 99% confidence interval for the mean.
FIG. 6
FIG. 6
Percentage acceptances for each nonword in Experiment 2 as a function of the log product probability.
FIG. 7
FIG. 7
Mean d′ within each constituent probability group (High vs Low) and length in syllables (two, three, or four) in Experiment 3. Error bars show 99% confidence interval for the mean.

References

    1. Aslin RN, Saffran JR, Newport EL. Computation of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science. 1998;9:321–324.
    1. Brown RW, Hildum DC. Expectancy and the perception of syllables. Language. 1956;32:411–419.
    1. Bybee JL. Morphology as lexical organization. In: Hammond M, Noonan M, editors. Theoretical morphology. San Diego: Academic Press; 1988. pp. 119–141.
    1. Chalmers KA, Humphreys MS. Role of generalized and episode specific memories in the word frequency effect in recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1998;24:610–632.
    1. Chalmers KA, Humphreys MS, Dennis S. A naturalistic study of the word frequency effect in episodic recognition. Memory & Cognition. 1997;25:780–784. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources