Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Nov 23:1:201.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00201. eCollection 2010.

Where do action goals come from? Evidence for spontaneous action-effect binding in infants

Affiliations

Where do action goals come from? Evidence for spontaneous action-effect binding in infants

Stephan Verschoor et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

One of the great questions in psychology concerns how we develop to become intentional agents. Ideomotor theory suggests that intentional actions depend on, and emerge from the automatic acquisition of bidirectional action-effect associations: perceiving an action-effect sequence creates an integrated representation that can be employed for action control in the opposite order, selecting an action by anticipating its effect. We provide first evidence for the spontaneous acquisition of bidirectional action-effect associations in 9- 12-, and 18-month-olds, suggesting that the mechanism underlying action-effect integration is in place at the latest around 9 months of age.

Keywords: action control; action representation; action/perception; action–effects; cognitive development; goal-directed action; infancy.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Experimental setup. The infant sat on the caretaker's lap while perceiving the stimulus events and pushing the touch-sensitive button that produced the action effect.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean Latencies (+SE) for 9-month-olds (n = 22), 12-month-olds (n = 21) and 18-month-olds (n = 22) to self-produced event and action-independent event.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean Percentage of responses (+SE) for 9-month-olds (n = 22), 12-month-olds (n = 21) and 18-month-olds (n = 22) to self-produced event and action-independent event.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Band G. P. H., Van Steenbergen H., Ridderinkhof K. R., Falkenstein M., Hommel B. (2009). Action-effect negativity: irrelevant action effects are monitored like relevant feedback. Biol. Psychol. 82, 211–21810.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.011 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Belsky J., Most R. K. (1981). Free exploration to play: a cross-sectional study of infant free play behavior. Dev. Psychol. 17, 630–63910.1037/0012-1649.17.5.630 - DOI
    1. Biro S., Leslie A. (2007). Infants’ perception of goal-directed actions. Development through cues-based bootstrapping. Dev. Sci. 10, 379–39810.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00544.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Carmody D. P., Dunn S. M., Boddie-Willis A. S., DeMarco J. K., Lewis M. (2004). A quantitative measure of myelination development in infants, using MR images. Neuroradiology 46, 781–78610.1007/s00234-004-1241-z - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. DeCasper A. J., Fifer W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science 1208, 1174–117610.1126/science.7375928 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources