Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2011;6(7):e22650.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022650. Epub 2011 Jul 28.

The second-agent effect: communicative gestures increase the likelihood of perceiving a second agent

Affiliations

The second-agent effect: communicative gestures increase the likelihood of perceiving a second agent

Valeria Manera et al. PLoS One. 2011.

Abstract

Background: Beyond providing cues about an agent's intention, communicative actions convey information about the presence of a second agent towards whom the action is directed (second-agent information). In two psychophysical studies we investigated whether the perceptual system makes use of this information to infer the presence of a second agent when dealing with impoverished and/or noisy sensory input.

Methodology/principal findings: Participants observed point-light displays of two agents (A and B) performing separate actions. In the Communicative condition, agent B's action was performed in response to a communicative gesture by agent A. In the Individual condition, agent A's communicative action was replaced with a non-communicative action. Participants performed a simultaneous masking yes-no task, in which they were asked to detect the presence of agent B. In Experiment 1, we investigated whether criterion c was lowered in the Communicative condition compared to the Individual condition, thus reflecting a variation in perceptual expectations. In Experiment 2, we manipulated the congruence between A's communicative gesture and B's response, to ascertain whether the lowering of c in the Communicative condition reflected a truly perceptual effect. Results demonstrate that information extracted from communicative gestures influences the concurrent processing of biological motion by prompting perception of a second agent (second-agent effect).

Conclusions/significance: We propose that this finding is best explained within a Bayesian framework, which gives a powerful rationale for the pervasive role of prior expectations in visual perception.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Example of a communicative signal trial.
Agent A points to an object to be picked up; agent B bends down and picks it up. B was presented using limited-lifetime technique (6 signal dots) and masked with temporally scrambled noise dots. The noise level displayed is the minimum allowed in the experiment (5 noise dots). To provide a static depiction of the animated sequence, dots extracted from 3 different frames are superimposed and simultaneously represented; the silhouette depicting the human form was not visible in the stimulus display.
Figure 2
Figure 2. SDT parameters for the two experimental conditions.
Error bars are 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 3
Figure 3. SDT parameters for the three experimental conditions.
Error bars are 95% confidence intervals.

References

    1. Bara BG. Cognitive Pragmatics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2010. 296
    1. Clark HH. Arenas of Language Use. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press; 1992. 440
    1. Grice HP. Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1989. 406
    1. Tomasello M. Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2008. 400
    1. Manera V, Becchio C, Schouten B, Bara BG, Verfaillie K. Communicative interactions improve visual detection of biological motion. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e14594. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types