Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Apr;1(2):182-189.
doi: 10.1177/1948550609359202.

More Memory Bang for the Attentional Buck: Self-Protection Goals Enhance Encoding Efficiency for Potentially Threatening Males

Affiliations

More Memory Bang for the Attentional Buck: Self-Protection Goals Enhance Encoding Efficiency for Potentially Threatening Males

D Vaughn Becker et al. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2010 Apr.

Abstract

When encountering individuals with a potential inclination to harm them, people face a dilemma: Staring at them provides useful information about their intentions but may also be perceived by them as intrusive and challenging-thereby increasing the likelihood of the very threat the people fear. One solution to this dilemma would be an enhanced ability to efficiently encode such individuals-to be able to remember them without spending any additional direct attention on them. In two experiments, the authors primed self-protective concerns in perceivers and assessed visual attention and recognition memory for a variety of faces. Consistent with hypotheses, self-protective participants (relative to control participants) exhibited enhanced encoding efficiency (i.e., greater memory not predicated on any enhancement of visual attention) for Black and Arab male faces-groups stereotyped as being potentially dangerous-but not for female or White male faces. Results suggest that encoding efficiency depends on the functional relevance of the social information people encounter.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or publication of this article.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean gaze duration (in seconds) as a function of face type and motivation condition (control vs. self-protection), Experiment 1
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean recognition accuracy (corrected for guessing) as a function of face type and motivation condition (control vs. self-protection), Experiment 1
Figure 3
Figure 3
This figure depicts the priming effects in Experiment 1—the mean in the self-protection condition minus the mean in the control condition—as estimated by the multilevel regression model including gaze duration as a varying covariate Note: The bars indicate 95% confidence intervals for the priming benefits (or costs) evaluated at the grand mean for gaze duration.
Figure 4
Figure 4
This figure depicts the priming effects in Experiment 2—the mean in the self-protection condition minus the mean in the control condition—as estimated by the multilevel regression model including gaze duration as a varying covariate Note: The bars indicate 95% confidence intervals for the priming benefits (or costs) evaluated at the grand mean for gaze duration.

References

    1. Anthony T, Copper C, Mullen B. Cross-racial facial identification: A social cognitive integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1992;18:296–301.
    1. Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM, Lee-Chai A, Barndollar K, Troetschel R. The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;81:1014–1027. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bargh JA, Williams EL. The automaticity of social life. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15:1–4. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bruner JS. On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review. 1957;64:123–152. - PubMed
    1. Daly M, Wilson M. Homicide. New York: Aldine de Gruyter; 1988.

LinkOut - more resources