Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2012 Aug 15;62(2):1086-91.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.026. Epub 2011 Sep 25.

Task-induced deactivation and the "resting" state

Affiliations
Review

Task-induced deactivation and the "resting" state

Jeffrey R Binder. Neuroimage. .

Abstract

Task-induced decreases in blood flow and the widespread use of "resting" baselines produced unexpected and discrepant results in early cognitive imaging studies, especially in language comprehension experiments. Here I describe from a personal perspective some of the events and thought processes leading to the first hypothesis-driven fMRI study of the "resting" state.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Overlap of regions showing “task-induced deactivation” and regions involved in semantic knowledge retrieval. Top row: Higher levels of BOLD signal during a “resting” state compared to a perceptual tone discrimination task. Bottom row: Higher levels of BOLD signal during a semantic decision task compared to a phonological control task. Overlapping regions include the medial temporal lobe, posterior cingulate gyrus, angular gyrus, and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Adapted from Binder et al. (1999).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Overlap of left hemisphere regions identified in a large-scale meta-analysis of 120 semantic imaging experiments (top) and regions showing task-induced deactivation in a comparison of “resting” and a perceptual tone discrimination task (bottom). Similarity between the maps is striking given that they are derived using very different types of data and analysis methods. Reprinted from Binder et al. (2009).

References

    1. Alexander MP, Hiltbrunner B, Fischer RS. Distributed anatomy of transcortical sensory aphasia. Archives of Neurology. 1989;46:885–892. - PubMed
    1. Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Watkins GL, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD. Remembering the past: Two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1995;152:1576–1585. - PubMed
    1. Andrews-Hanna JR. The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist. 2011 in press. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Antrobus JS. Information theory and stimulus-independent thought. British Journal of Psychology. 1968;59:423–430.
    1. Antrobus JS, Singer JL, Greenberg S. Studies in the stream of consciousness: Experimental enhancement and suppression of spontaneous cognitive processes. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1966;23:399–417.