Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2011;6(11):e27926.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027926. Epub 2011 Nov 21.

Learned value magnifies salience-based attentional capture

Affiliations

Learned value magnifies salience-based attentional capture

Brian A Anderson et al. PLoS One. 2011.

Abstract

Visual attention is captured by physically salient stimuli (termed salience-based attentional capture), and by otherwise task-irrelevant stimuli that contain goal-related features (termed contingent attentional capture). Recently, we reported that physically nonsalient stimuli associated with value through reward learning also capture attention involuntarily (Anderson, Laurent, & Yantis, PNAS, 2011). Although it is known that physical salience and goal-relatedness both influence attentional priority, it is unknown whether or how attentional capture by a salient stimulus is modulated by its associated value. Here we show that a physically salient, task-irrelevant distractor previously associated with a large reward slows visual search more than an equally salient distractor previously associated with a smaller reward. This magnification of salience-based attentional capture by learned value extinguishes over several hundred trials. These findings reveal a broad influence of learned value on involuntary attentional capture.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Behavioral Task.
Sequence of events and time course for a trial during training (a) and at test (b) in Experiment 1.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Behavioral Results for the Training Phase of Experiment 1.
Mean response time ± within-subjects s.e.m. for high- and low-reward targets over the course of the training phase. Only the main effect of trial block was significant [F(9,153) = 4.92, p<.001, ηp 2 = .224].
Figure 3
Figure 3. Behavioral Results for the Test Phase of Experiment 1.
Mean response time ± within-subjects s.e.m. for each distractor condition over the course of the test phase. The difference in RT on trials containing a high-value vs. a low-value distractor represents the effect of learned value on salience-driven attentional capture.
Figure 4
Figure 4. Behavioral Results for the Test Phase of Experiment 2.
Mean response time ± within-subjects s.e.m. for each distractor condition over the course of the test phase.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215. - PubMed
    1. Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995;18:193–222. - PubMed
    1. Serences JT, Shomstein S, Leber AB, Golay X, Egeth HE, et al. Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex. Psychol Sci. 2005;16:114–122. - PubMed
    1. Serences JT, Yantis S. Representation of attentional priority in human occipital, parietal, and frontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:284–293. - PubMed
    1. Laurent PA. The emergence of saliency and novelty responses from reinforcement learning principles. Neural Netw. 2008;21:1493–1499. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types