Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Mar;18(2):286-94.
doi: 10.1017/S135561771100169X. Epub 2011 Dec 15.

Lifestyle activities and memory: variety may be the spice of life. The women's health and aging study II

Affiliations

Lifestyle activities and memory: variety may be the spice of life. The women's health and aging study II

Michelle C Carlson et al. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012 Mar.

Abstract

This study examined whether participation in a variety of lifestyle activities was comparable to frequent participation in cognitively challenging activities in mitigating impairments in cognitive abilities susceptible to aging in healthy, community-dwelling older women. Frequencies of participation in various lifestyle activities on the Lifestyle Activities Questionnaire (LAQ) were divided according to high (e.g., reading), moderate (e.g., discussing politics), and low (e.g., watching television) cognitive demand. We also considered the utility of participation in a variety of lifestyle activities regardless of cognitive challenge. Immediate and delayed verbal recall, psychomotor speed, and executive function were each measured at baseline and at five successive exams, spanning a 9.5-year interval. Greater variety of participation in activities, regardless of cognitive challenge, was associated with an 8 to 11% reduction in the risk of impairment in verbal memory and global cognitive outcomes. Participation in a variety of lifestyle activities was more predictive than frequency or level of cognitive challenge for significant reductions in risk of incident impairment on measures sensitive to cognitive aging and risk for dementia. Our findings offer new perspectives in promoting a diverse repertoire of activities to mitigate age-related cognitive declines.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Box plots of 23 everyday activities rank-ordered by a panel of cognitive psychologists according to relative level of cognitive demand, from least (left) to most (right) demanding.

References

    1. Aartsen MJ, Smits CH, van Tilberg T, Knipscheer KC, Deeg DJ. Activity in older adults: Cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study of everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2002;57:P153–P162. - PubMed
    1. Arbuckle TY, Maag U, Pushkar D, Chaikelson J. Individual differences in trajectory of intellectual development over 45 years of adulthood. Psychology & Aging. 1998;13:663–675. - PubMed
    1. Arbuckle TY, Pushkar Gold D, Chaikelson JS, Lapidus S. Measurement of activity in the elderly: The activities checklist. Canadian Journal on Aging. 1994;13:550–565.
    1. Baltes PB, Lindenberger U, Staudinger UM. Lifespan theory in developmental psychology. In: Lerner RM, editor. Theoretical models of human development. Handbook of child psychology. 5th ed. New York: Wiley; 1997.
    1. Baltes PB, Willis SL. Plasticity and enhancement of intellectual functioning in old age: Penn State’s Adult Development and Enrichment Project (ADEPT) In: Craik FIM, Trehub S, editors. Aging and cognitive processes. New York: Plenum Press; 1982.

Publication types