Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Feb 14;109(7):2613-8.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112623109. Epub 2012 Jan 30.

Foraging alters resilience/vulnerability to sleep disruption and starvation in Drosophila

Affiliations

Foraging alters resilience/vulnerability to sleep disruption and starvation in Drosophila

Jeffrey Donlea et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

Recent human studies suggest that genetic polymorphisms allow an individual to maintain optimal cognitive functioning during sleep deprivation. If such polymorphisms were not associated with additional costs, selective pressures would allow these alleles to spread through the population such that an evolutionary alternative to sleep would emerge. To determine whether there are indeed costs associated with resiliency to sleep loss, we challenged natural allelic variants of the foraging gene (for) with either sleep deprivation or starvation. Flies with high levels of Protein Kinase G (PKG) (for(R)) do not display deficits in short-term memory following 12 h of sleep deprivation. However, short-term memory is significantly disrupted when for(R) flies are starved overnight. In contrast, flies with low levels of PKG (for(s), for(s2)) show substantial deficits in short-term memory following sleep deprivation but retain their ability to learn after 12 h of starvation. We found that for(R) phenotypes could be largely recapitulated in for(s) flies by selectively increasing the level of PKG in the α/β lobes of the mushroom bodies, a structure known to regulate both sleep and memory. Together, these data indicate that whereas the expression of for may appear to provide resilience in one environmental context, it may confer an unexpected vulnerability in other situations. Understanding how these tradeoffs confer resilience or vulnerability to specific environmental challenges may provide additional clues as to why an evolutionary alternative to sleep has not emerged.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
foraging differentially confers resilience/vulnerability to SD and starvation. (A) forR flies do not compensate for 12 h of SD with a subsequent increase in sleep, whereas both fors and fors2 mutants exhibit a sleep rebound typical of Cs flies. % sleep recovered is calculated for each individual as a ratio of the minutes of sleep gained above baseline during 48 h of recovery divided by the minutes of sleep lost during SD; *P < 0.05, modified Bonferroni test. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. (B) STM is impaired in fors flies following 12 h of SD, whereas fors2 mutants display impairments both during baseline and following SD. In contrast, forR flies maintain normal STM following SD. (C) When placed in starvation before lights out, forR flies display significantly less sleep than during the previous baseline night and do not exhibit a sleep rebound when placed back onto food the following morning. Neither fors flies nor fors2 mutants respond to starvation with an increase in waking. Cumulative sleep lost or gained during starvation. (D) STM is impaired in forR flies when waking is induced by starvation. fors flies maintain STM following a night of starvation. Surprisingly, fors2 mutants have normal STM following a night of starvation. (E) STM is impaired in fors/fors2 flies under baseline conditions compared with starved siblings. Sleep-deprived fors/fors2 flies show no further STM impairments relative to baseline. (F) fors flies survive longer than forR and fors2 flies during chronic starvation (n > 27 per group).
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Sleep deprivation does not block LTM in forR flies. (A) forR females increase sleep following social enrichment relative to isolated siblings, whereas fors and fors2 females do not. *P < 0.05, modified Bonferroni test. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. (B) forR flies have intact LTM that is not disrupted when sleep-deprived for 4 h immediately after training (T+SD). Trained fors flies have intact LTM during baseline but are impaired when sleep-deprived following training. fors2 mutants have impaired LTM; courtship was not evaluated in fors2 flies following SD (ND). NS, nonsignificant. *P < 0.05, modified Bonferroni test. (C) Following spaced training for courtship conditioning, forR males, but not fors or fors2 flies, sleep significantly more than their naïve siblings; *P = 0.03.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Expressing foraging in the MB confers resilience to sleep deprivation. (AC) When UAS-for is expressed using c739-GAL4/fors (A) and 30y-GAL4/fors (B) drivers, no sleep rebound is observed after 12 h of SD. Parental lines display a sleep rebound. Interestingly, w; fors,201y/fors;UAS-for flies (C) and their parental controls exhibit a sleep rebound; *P < 0.05, modified Bonferroni test. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. n.s., nonsignificant. (D and E) As expected, w; fors,c739/fors, w; fors;UAS-for/+, and w; fors;30y parental lines display significant reductions in STM following 12 h of SD, whereas both w; fors,c739/fors;UAS-for/+ (D) and w; fors;30y/UAS-for (E) flies retain STM following SD. (F) During baseline, UAS-dicer2/+;30y/UAS-forRNAi flies show deficits in STM, whereas both parental controls learn; performance remains low after SD in all genotypes. However, UAS-dicer2/+;30y/UAS-forRNAi flies display STM following starvation. (GI) Courtship conditioning fails to induce LTM in w; fors,c739/fors;UAS-for/+ (G) and w; fors;30y/UAS-for (H) but is intact in the parental lines. w; fors,201y/fors;UAS-for/+ flies and parental controls have intact LTM (I).
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
for overexpression in the MBs alters response to starvation. (AC) w; fors,c739/fors;UAS-for/+ and w; fors;30y/UAS-for flies exhibit a forR-like response to 15 h of starvation, whereas the parental lines retain the fors phenotype; fors;UAS-for/+ data are replotted in B and C to facilitate comparisons. In contrast, w; fors,201y/fors;UAS-for/+ flies and their parental controls (w; fors,201y/fors) exhibit a fors response to starvation (C). Data are presented as mean ± SEM. (DF) Survival during chronic starvation is not altered when UAS-for is expressed in MB α and β lobes but is increased when UAS-for is expressed in MB γ lobes.

References

    1. Van Dongen HPA, Baynard MD, Maislin G, Dinges DF. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: Evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep. 2004;27:423–433. - PubMed
    1. Viola AU, et al. PER3 polymorphism predicts sleep structure and waking performance. Curr Biol. 2007;17:613–618. - PubMed
    1. Osborne KA, et al. Natural behavior polymorphism due to a cGMP-dependent protein kinase of Drosophila. Science. 1997;277:834–836. - PubMed
    1. Fitzpatrick MJ, Feder E, Rowe L, Sokolowski MB. Maintaining a behaviour polymorphism by frequency-dependent selection on a single gene. Nature. 2007;447:210–212. - PubMed
    1. Sokolowski MB. Social interactions in “simple” model systems. Neuron. 2010;65:780–794. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources