Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Feb 28;109(9):3253-8.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113380109. Epub 2012 Feb 13.

At 6-9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns

Affiliations

At 6-9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns

Elika Bergelson et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

It is widely accepted that infants begin learning their native language not by learning words, but by discovering features of the speech signal: consonants, vowels, and combinations of these sounds. Learning to understand words, as opposed to just perceiving their sounds, is said to come later, between 9 and 15 mo of age, when infants develop a capacity for interpreting others' goals and intentions. Here, we demonstrate that this consensus about the developmental sequence of human language learning is flawed: in fact, infants already know the meanings of several common words from the age of 6 mo onward. We presented 6- to 9-mo-old infants with sets of pictures to view while their parent named a picture in each set. Over this entire age range, infants directed their gaze to the named pictures, indicating their understanding of spoken words. Because the words were not trained in the laboratory, the results show that even young infants learn ordinary words through daily experience with language. This surprising accomplishment indicates that, contrary to prevailing beliefs, either infants can already grasp the referential intentions of adults at 6 mo or infants can learn words before this ability emerges. The precocious discovery of word meanings suggests a perspective in which learning vocabulary and learning the sound structure of spoken language go hand in hand as language acquisition begins.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Experimental setup. The child sat on her parent's lap and was presented with images and sounds from a computer equipped with an eye tracker and speakers. The experimenter sat behind a screen and was not visible to the infant. The experimenter controlled presentation of stimuli and monitored the child on a live-feed camera. A backup video recording of the session was made to allow for confirmation of the validity of gross characteristics of the eye-tracking data stream. The figure shows an example of images presented on a paired-picture trial testing “banana” or “hair.”
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Subject and item-pair means for 6- to 7- and 8- to 9-mo-olds. All data (AH) were calculated over a window from 367 to 3,500 ms post target word onset. Subject mean difference scores are shown for paired-picture trials for 6- to 7-mo-olds (A) and for 8- to 9-mo-olds (B). Subject mean increases in target looking, corrected for baseline looking, are displayed for scene trials for 6- to 7-mo-olds (C) and for 8- to 9-mo-olds (D). Item-pair mean difference scores are shown for paired-picture trials for 6- to 7-mo-olds (E) and for 8- to 9-mo-olds (F). Item mean increases in target looking, corrected for baseline looking, are given for scene trials for 6- to 7-mo-olds (G) and for 8- to 9-mo-olds (H). (EH) Error bars represent bootstrapped nonparametric 95% confidence intervals. On the right of each subplot is a histogram of the responses in the main plot; all histograms show more positive than negative responses for each subset of subjects and of item pairs.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Time course of infants’ picture fixation on paired-picture trials, averaged over infants in four age groups. The ordinate shows the mean proportion of infants who were looking at the named (target) picture at each moment in time. Error bars indicate SEMs, with means computed over subjects in each age range. At all four ages, target fixation rose from about 0.50 (chance) shortly after the onset of the spoken word. Overall, accuracy in fixating the named picture increased with age across the age groups. See SI Text for further details.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Polka L, Werker JF. Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1994;20:421–435. - PubMed
    1. Werker JF, Tees RC. Cross-language speech-perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the 1st year of life. Infant Behav Dev. 1984;7:49–63.
    1. Kuhl PK, Williams KA, Lacerda F, Stevens KN, Lindblom B. Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science. 1992;255:606–608. - PubMed
    1. Gervain J, Mehler J. Speech perception and language acquisition in the first year of life. Annu Rev Psychol. 2010;61:191–218. - PubMed
    1. Jusczyk PW, Aslin RN. Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognit Psychol. 1995;29:1–23. - PubMed

Publication types