Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Sep;67(5):563-71.
doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr153. Epub 2012 Feb 24.

The impact of age stereotypes on self-perceptions of aging across the adult lifespan

Affiliations

The impact of age stereotypes on self-perceptions of aging across the adult lifespan

Dana Kotter-Grühn et al. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2012 Sep.

Abstract

Objectives: Individuals' perceptions of their own age(ing) are important correlates of well-being and health. The goals of the present study were to (a) examine indicators of self-perceptions of aging across adulthood and (b) experimentally test whether age stereotypes influence self-perceptions of aging.

Method: Adults 18-92 years of age were presented with positive, negative, or no age stereotypes. Before and after the stereotype activation, aging satisfaction and subjective age were measured.

Results: The activation of positive age stereotypes did not positively influence self-perceptions of aging. Quite the contrary, priming middle-aged and older adults in good health with positive age stereotypes made them feel older. After the activation of negative age stereotypes, older adults in good health felt older and those in bad health wanted to be younger than before the priming. Even younger and middle-aged adults reported younger desired ages after the negative age stereotype priming. Persons in bad health also thought they looked older after being primed with negative age stereotypes.

Discussion: Taken together, although we find some support for contrast effects, most of our results can be interpreted in terms of assimilation effects, suggesting that individuals integrate stereotypical information into their self-evaluations of age(ing) when confronted with stereotypes.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Change in felt age for older adults in above average health.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Change in desired age for persons in below average health.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Change in perceived age for persons in below average health.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Dijksterhuis A, Bargh JA. The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In: Zanna MP, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 33. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2001. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(01)80003-4.
    1. Dijksterhuis A, Spears R, Lépinasse V. Reflecting and deflecting stereotypes: Assimilation and contrast in impression formation and automatic behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2001;37:286–299. doi:10.1006/jesp.2000.1449.
    1. Eibach RP, Mock SE, Courtney EA. Having a “senior moment”: Induced aging phenomenology, subjective age, and susceptibility to ageist stereotypes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2010;46:643–649. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.002.
    1. Filipp S.-H., Ferring D, Klauer T. Subjektives Alterserleben: Ein Merkmal erfolgreichen Alterns? [Subjective age experience: A characteristicum of successful aging?] In: Baltes MM, editor. Erfolgreiches Altern: Bedingungen und Variationen. Bern, Switzerland: Huber; 1989. pp. 296–300.
    1. Galambos NL, Turner PK, Tilton-Weaver LC. Chronological and subjective age in emerging adulthood: The crossover effect. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2005;20:538–556. doi:10.1177/074355840527487.

Publication types