Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2012 Jun 25;106(4):423-7.
doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.02.023. Epub 2012 Feb 28.

A comparison of low- and high-impact forced exercise: effects of training paradigm on learning and memory

Affiliations
Comparative Study

A comparison of low- and high-impact forced exercise: effects of training paradigm on learning and memory

John A Kennard et al. Physiol Behav. .

Abstract

In this study we compared two types of forced exercise-a low impact paradigm to minimize stress, which included speeds up to 10 m/min and a stressful high impact paradigm, with speeds up to 21 m/min. 150 male C57BL/6J mice were randomly assigned to the low impact, high impact, or sedentary control conditions and were tested on the rotorod and Morris water maze (MWM) as indices of motor learning and spatial memory. We found that 5 weeks of stressful high speed forced exercise led to significant improvement in rotorod performance, as high impact runners outperformed both low impact runners and controls at 15 and 25 rpm speeds. These differences were the result of improved physical fitness due to exercise and likely do not reflect enhanced learning in these mice. In the MWM, 5 weeks of stressful high impact exercise led to significant impairment in spatial memory acquisition compared to low impact runners and controls. Low impact exercise for 10 weeks significantly improved retention of spatial memory compared to high impact exercise. Results suggested that these two paradigms produced different effects of forced exercise on learning and memory. The low impact paradigm led to some improvements, whereas the stressful high impact program caused significant impairment. Comparison of these two paradigms begins to address the window between the beneficial and detrimental effects of forced exercise, and have suggested a boundary of exercise intensity that leads to impairment in learning.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Rotorod performance in mice randomly assigned to a low impact exercise, high impact exercise or sedentary control condition tested at 15 and 25 revolutions per minute (RPM) over three days. At both speeds, learning is expressed by all groups. Overall, high impact runners had significantly longer latencies to fall than both controls and low impact runners at 15 RPM (ps < .01) and 25 RPM (ps < .001). . * high impact runners significantly different from control and low impact (p < .05).
Figure 2
Figure 2
MWM acquisition in mice randomly assigned to a low impact exercise, high impact exercise or sedentary control condition. Overall, all groups showed evidence of learning in that latency to escape decreased across nine Training Sessions. High impact runners had significantly longer latencies to escape compared to low impact runners and sedentary controls in Training Sessions 5 and 7. Values are mean ± SEM. ‡ high impact runners significantly different from controls (p < .01); high impact runners significantly different than low impact runners (* p < .05; ** p < .01).
Figure 3
Figure 3
MWM retention in mice randomly assigned to a low impact exercise, high impact exercise or sedentary control condition. All mice crossed the area in the trained platform quadrant more times than the same area in untrained adjacent and opposite quadrants. Within the trained quadrant, low impact runners crossed the former platform location significantly more than high impact runners. Values are mean ± SEM. ** low impact runners significantly different from high impact runners (p < .01).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Haskell W, Lee IM, Pate RR, Powell KE, Blair SN, Franklin BA, et al. Physical activity and public health: Updated recommendation for adults from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2007;39:1423–1434. - PubMed
    1. Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, et al. Aerobic training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61(11):1166–1170. - PubMed
    1. Tomporowski PD, Davis CL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Exercise and Children's Intelligence, 714 Cognition, and Academic Achievement. Educ Psychol Rev. 2008;20:111–131. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Hotting K, Reich B, Holzschneider K, Kauschke K, Schmidt T, Reer R, et al. Differential cognitive effects of cycling versus stretching/coordination training in middle-aged adults. Health Psychol 2011. 2011 - PubMed
    1. Kramer AF, Hahn S, Cohen NJ, Banich MT, McAuley E, Harrison CR, et al. Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature. 1999;400:418–419. - PubMed

Publication types