Sensorimotor integration during stance: processing time of active or passive addition or withdrawal of visual or haptic information
- PMID: 22516013
- DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.03.044
Sensorimotor integration during stance: processing time of active or passive addition or withdrawal of visual or haptic information
Abstract
Vision (V) and touch (T) help stabilize our standing body, but little is known on the time-interval necessary for the brain to process the sensory inflow (or its removal) and exploit the new information (or counteract its removal). We have estimated the latency of onset and the time-course of the changes in postural control mode following addition or withdrawal of sensory information and the effect of anticipation thereof. Ten subjects stood in tandem position. They wore LCD goggles that allowed or removed vision, or lightly touched (eyes-closed) with the index finger (haptic stimulation) a pad that could be suddenly lowered (passive task). In different sessions, sensory shifts were deliberately produced by opening (or closing) the eyes or touching the pad (or lifting the finger) (active task). We recorded eyelid movement and finger force (<1N), sway of center of foot pressure (CoP), electromyogram (EMG) of soleus, tibialis and peroneus muscle, bilaterally, and of extensor indicis. The latency of the CoP and EMG changes following the shifts were statistically estimated on the averaged traces of 50 repetitions per condition. Muscle activity and sway adaptively decreased in amplitude on adding stabilizing visual or haptic information. The time-interval from the sensory shift to decrease in EMG and sway was ∼0.5-2 s under both conditions. It was shorter for tibialis than peroneus or soleus and shorter for visual than haptic shift. CoP followed the tibialis by ∼0.2 s. Slightly shorter intervals were observed following active sensory shifts. Latencies of EMG and postural changes were the shortest on removal of both haptic and visual information. Subsequently, the time taken to reach the steady-state was ∼1-3 s under both active and passive tasks. A startle response at ∼100 ms could precede EMG changes. Reaction-time contractions in response to sensory shifts appeared at ∼200 ms, earlier than the adaptive changes. Changes in postural behavior require a finite amount of time from visual or haptic shift, much longer than reflexes or rapid voluntary responses, suggesting a time-consuming central integration process. This process is longer on addition than removal of haptic information, indicating a heavier computational load. These findings should be taken into account when considering problems of sensorimotor integration in elderly subjects or patients and when designing simulation models of human balance.
Copyright © 2012 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Similar articles
-
Sensori-motor integration during stance: time adaptation of control mechanisms on adding or removing vision.Hum Mov Sci. 2011 Apr;30(2):172-89. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2010.06.002. Epub 2010 Aug 19. Hum Mov Sci. 2011. PMID: 20727610
-
Processing time of addition or withdrawal of single or combined balance-stabilizing haptic and visual information.J Neurophysiol. 2015 Dec;114(6):3097-110. doi: 10.1152/jn.00618.2015. Epub 2015 Sep 2. J Neurophysiol. 2015. PMID: 26334013 Free PMC article.
-
Adaptation to continuous perturbation of balance: progressive reduction of postural muscle activity with invariant or increasing oscillations of the center of mass depending on perturbation frequency and vision conditions.Hum Mov Sci. 2011 Apr;30(2):262-78. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2011.02.002. Epub 2011 Mar 25. Hum Mov Sci. 2011. PMID: 21440318
-
Time-interval for integration of stabilizing haptic and visual information in subjects balancing under static and dynamic conditions.Front Syst Neurosci. 2014 Oct 6;8:190. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00190. eCollection 2014. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014. PMID: 25339872 Free PMC article. Review.
-
Light touch for balance: influence of a time-varying external driving signal.Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011 Nov 12;366(1581):3133-41. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0169. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011. PMID: 21969695 Free PMC article. Review.
Cited by
-
The effects of visual and haptic vertical stimulation on standing balance in stroke patients.Ann Rehabil Med. 2013 Dec;37(6):862-70. doi: 10.5535/arm.2013.37.6.862. Epub 2013 Dec 23. Ann Rehabil Med. 2013. PMID: 24466521 Free PMC article.
-
Mechanisms of interpersonal sway synchrony and stability.J R Soc Interface. 2014 Dec 6;11(101):20140751. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2014.0751. J R Soc Interface. 2014. PMID: 25339686 Free PMC article.
-
Incongruity of Geometric and Spectral Markers in the Assessment of Body Sway.Front Neurol. 2022 Jul 18;13:929132. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.929132. eCollection 2022. Front Neurol. 2022. PMID: 35923830 Free PMC article.
-
Haptic Cues for Balance: Use of a Cane Provides Immediate Body Stabilization.Front Neurosci. 2017 Dec 14;11:705. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00705. eCollection 2017. Front Neurosci. 2017. PMID: 29311785 Free PMC article.
-
Sensory reweighting dynamics in human postural control.J Neurophysiol. 2014 May;111(9):1852-64. doi: 10.1152/jn.00669.2013. Epub 2014 Feb 5. J Neurophysiol. 2014. PMID: 24501263 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources