Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Jan;21(1):85-106.
doi: 10.1002/icd.764. Epub 2011 Nov 14.

The Association between Positive Parenting and Externalizing Behavior

Affiliations

The Association between Positive Parenting and Externalizing Behavior

Debra L Boeldt et al. Infant Child Dev. 2012 Jan.

Abstract

The present study examined the role of positive parenting on externalizing behaviors in a longitudinal, genetically informative sample. It often is assumed that positive parenting prevents behavior problems in children via an environmentally mediated process. Alternatively, the association may be due to either an evocative gene-environment correlation, in which parents react to children's genetically-influenced behavior in a positive way, or a passive gene-environment correlation, where parents passively transmit a risk environment and the genetic risk factor for the behavioral outcome to their children. The present study estimated the contribution of these processes in the association between positive parenting and children's externalizing behavior. Positive parenting was assessed via observations at ages 7, 9, 14, 24, and 36 months and externalizing behaviors were assessed through parent report at ages 4, 5, 7, 9, 10, 11, and 12 years. The significant association between positive parenting and externalizing behavior was negative, with children of mothers who showed significantly more positive parenting during toddlerhood having lower levels of externalizing behavior in childhood; however, there was not adequate power to distinguish whether this covariation was due to genetic, shared environmental, or nonshared environmental influences.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Results of confirmatory factor analyses with separate factors for Positive Parenting and Externalizing Behavior. sense = maternal sensitivity; quality = maternal quality of instruction; warmth = maternal warmth; interact= overall rating of mother–child interaction. Results for the full sample are presented by the top numbers, results for boys are presented by the middle numbers, and results for girls are presented by the bottoms numbers. All parenting variables had significant loadings (p < .01) on the latent parenting factor, and all externalizing behavior variables had significant loadings (p < .01) on the latent externalizing behavior factor. * p<.01
Figure 2
Figure 2
Results of the full bivariate common pathway model with separate factors for Positive Parenting and Externalizing Behavior for the entire sample. sense = maternal sensitivity; quality = maternal quality of instruction; warmth = maternal warmth; interact= overall rating of mother–child interaction; A=magnitude of genetic influences; C= magnitude of shared environmental influences; E= nonshared environmental influences on positive parenting, externalizing behavior, and the covariance between positive parenting and externalizing behavior.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Results of the full bivariate common pathway model with separate factors for Positive Parenting and Externalizing Behavior for boys and girls. sense = maternal sensitivity; quality = maternal quality of instruction; warmth = maternal warmth; interact= overall rating of mother–child interaction; A=magnitude of genetic influences; C= magnitude of shared environmental influences; E= nonshared environmental influences on positive parenting, externalizing behavior, and the covariance between positive parenting and externalizing behavior. Top/left number indicates boys and bottom/right number indicates girls.

References

    1. Achenbach TM. Manual of the Child Behavior Checklist 4–18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991.
    1. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;107:238–246. - PubMed
    1. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Infant temperament, parenting, and externalizing behavior in first grade: a test of the differential hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;49:124–131. - PubMed
    1. Brestan EV, Eyberg SM. Effective psychosocial treatments of conduct-disordered children and adolescents: 29 years, 82 studies, and 5,272 kids. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1998;27:180–189. - PubMed
    1. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. pp. 136–162.