Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review

Multisensory Integration Develops Late in Humans

In: The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2012. Chapter 18.
Affiliations
Free Books & Documents
Review

Multisensory Integration Develops Late in Humans

David Burr et al.
Free Books & Documents

Excerpt

Recent studies suggest that human infants can transfer information gleaned from one sense to another (e.g., Streri 2003; Streri et al. 2004). For example, 1-month-old infants can visually recognize an object they have previously explored orally (Gibson and Walker 1984; Meltzoff and Borton 1979) and 2-month-old infants can visually recognize an object they have previously felt (Rose 1981; Streri et al. 2008). However, many of these studies show an asymmetry in the transfer (Sann and Streri 2007; Streri 2003; Streri et al. 2008) or a partial dominance of one modality over another (Lewkowicz 1988a, 1988b), supporting the idea that, even when multimodal skills are present, they are not necessarily fully mature. Recent results (Bremner et al. 2008a, 2008b) on the representation of peripersonal space support the presence of two distinct mechanisms in sensory integration with different developmental trends: the first, relying principally on visual information, is present during the first 6 months; the second, incorporating information of hand and body posture with visual, develops only after 6.5 months of age.

Over the past years, the majority of multisensory studies in infants and children have investigated the development of multisensory matching, transfer, and facilitation abilities, whereas few of those have investigated the development of multisensory integration. Those few that did investigate multisensory integration in school-age children point to unimodal dominance rather than integration abilities (Hatwell 1987; Klein 1966; McGurk and Power 1980; Misceo et al. 1999).

PubMed Disclaimer

References

    1. Alais D, Burr D. The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration. Current Biology. 2004;14:257–62. - PubMed
    1. Alary F, Duquette M, Goldstein R, Elaine Chapman C, Voss P, La Buissonniere-Ariza V, Lepore F. Tactile acuity in the blind: A closer look reveals superiority over the sighted in some but not all cutaneous tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2037–43. - PubMed
    1. Atkinson J. The developing visual brain. New York: Oxford Univ. Press; 2000.
    1. Bahrick L.E. Increasing specificity in perceptual development: Infants' detection of nested levels of multimodal stimulation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2001;79:253–70. - PubMed
    1. Bahrick L.E, Flom R, Lickliter R. Intersensory redundancy facilitates discrimination of tempo in 3-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology. 2002;41:352–63. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources