Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012;7(5):e37449.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037449. Epub 2012 May 18.

Spatial reorientation by geometry in bumblebees

Affiliations

Spatial reorientation by geometry in bumblebees

Valeria Anna Sovrano et al. PLoS One. 2012.

Abstract

Human and non-human animals are capable of using basic geometric information to reorient in an environment. Geometric information includes metric properties associated with spatial surfaces (e.g., short vs. long wall) and left-right directionality or 'sense' (e.g. a long wall to the left of a short wall). However, it remains unclear whether geometric information is encoded by explicitly computing the layout of surface geometry or by matching images of the environment. View-based spatial encoding is generally thought to hold for insect navigation and, very recently, evidence for navigation by geometry has been reported in ants but only in a condition which does not allow the animals to use features located far from the goal. In this study we tested the spatial reorientation abilities of bumblebees (Bombus terrestris). After spatial disorientation, by passive rotation both clockwise and anticlockwise, bumblebees had to find one of the four exit holes located in the corners of a rectangular enclosure. Bumblebees systematically confused geometrically equivalent exit corners (i.e. corners with the same geometric arrangement of metric properties and sense, for example a short wall to the left of a long wall). However, when one wall of the enclosure was a different colour, bumblebees appeared to combine this featural information (either near or far from the goal) with geometric information to find the correct exit corner. Our results show that bumblebees are able to use both geometric and featural information to reorient themselves, even when features are located far from the goal.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Schematic view of the test apparatus.
Corners are conventionally identified as C = Correct, R = Rotation (geometrically equivalent corner), F = Far, N = Near. Only the external hole of the correct corner was opened allowing bumblebees to exit.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Top panels: the mean number of first choices made to each corner by the group of bumblebees (Exp. 1; groups means with SEM are shown) per session.
Bottom panels: the mean number of times (choice frequency) the same bees (group means with SEM are shown) visited each corner per session.
Figure 3
Figure 3. (A) The mean number of first choices made to each corner by the group of bumblebees (Exp. 2; groups means with SEM are shown) per session.
Top panels: Near-feature: the feature (white wall indicated by the horizontal grey stripe, all other walls are green) is located at the target corner C; Bottom panels: Far-feature: the feature (white wall indicated by the horizontal grey stripe, all other walls are green) is located away from the target corner C. (B) The mean number of times (choice frequency) bumblebees visited each corner (Exp. 2; groups means with SEM are shown) per session. Top panels: Near-feature: the feature (white wall indicated by the horizontal grey stripe, all other walls are green) is located at the target corner C; Bottom panels: Far-feature: the feature (white wall indicated by the horizontal grey stripe, all other walls are green) is located away from the target corner C.
Figure 4
Figure 4. The mean number of times (choice frequency) bumblebees visited each corner (group means with SEM are shown).
Data for the two testing sessions were pooled because there was no significant interaction in the general ANOVA between distance from the feature and session (Exp. 2). Left panel: Near-feature: the feature (white wall indicated by the horizontal grey stripe, all other walls are green) is located at the target corner C. Right panel: Far-feature: the feature (white wall indicated by the horizontal grey stripe, all other walls are green) is located away from the target corner C.

References

    1. Cheng K. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation. Cognition. 1986;23:149–178. - PubMed
    1. Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G. Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: Encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish. Cognition. 2002;85:B51–B59. - PubMed
    1. Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G. Modularity as a fish views it: conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial reorientation. J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process. 2003;29:199–210. - PubMed
    1. Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G. Animals' use of landmarks and metric information to reorient: effects of the size of the experimental space. Cognition. 2005;97:121–133. - PubMed
    1. Vargas JP, Lopez JC, Salas C, Thinus-Blanc C. Encoding of geometric and featural spatial information by Goldfish (Carassius auratus). J Comp Psychol. 2004;118:206–216. - PubMed

Publication types

MeSH terms