Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Jul 1;35(7):997-1002.
doi: 10.5665/sleep.1968.

Acute versus chronic partial sleep deprivation in middle-aged people: differential effect on performance and sleepiness

Affiliations

Acute versus chronic partial sleep deprivation in middle-aged people: differential effect on performance and sleepiness

Pierre Philip et al. Sleep. .

Abstract

Study objective: To evaluate the effects of acute sleep deprivation and chronic sleep restriction on vigilance, performance, and self-perception of sleepiness.

Design: Habitual night followed by 1 night of total sleep loss (acute sleep deprivation) or 5 consecutive nights of 4 hr of sleep (chronic sleep restriction) and recovery night.

Participants: Eighteen healthy middle-aged male participants (age [(± standard deviation] = 49.7 ± 2.6 yr, range 46-55 yr).

Measurements: Multiple sleep latency test trials, Karolinska Sleepiness Scale scores, simple reaction time test (lapses and 10% fastest reaction times), and nocturnal polysomnography data were recorded.

Results: Objective and subjective sleepiness increased immediately in response to sleep restriction. Sleep latencies after the second and third nights of sleep restriction reached levels equivalent to those observed after acute sleep deprivation, whereas Karolinska Sleepiness Scale scores did not reach these levels. Lapse occurrence increased after the second day of sleep restriction and reached levels equivalent to those observed after acute sleep deprivation. A statistical model revealed that sleepiness and lapses did not progressively worsen across days of sleep restriction. Ten percent fastest reaction times (i.e., optimal alertness) were not affected by acute or chronic sleep deprivation. Recovery to baseline levels of alertness and performance occurred after 8-hr recovery night.

Conclusions: In middle-aged study participants, sleep restriction induced a high increase in sleep propensity but adaptation to chronic sleep restriction occurred beyond day 3 of restriction. This sleepiness attenuation was underestimated by the participants. One recovery night restores daytime sleepiness and cognitive performance deficits induced by acute or chronic sleep deprivation.

Citation: Philip P; Sagaspe P; Prague M; Tassi P; Capelli A; Bioulac B; Commenges D; Taillard J. Acute versus chronic partial sleep deprivation in middle-aged people: differential effect on performance and sleepiness. SLEEP 2012;35(7):997-1002.

Keywords: Acute and chronic sleep deprivation; cognitive performance; sleepiness.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean number of lapses (A), mean sleep latencies on the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) (B), and mean Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) scores (C) (mean ± standard error of the mean) across days after Baseline night (B), Acute Sleep Deprivation (A SD), Recovery night after acute sleep deprivation (Ra), 5 consecutive Sleep Restriction nights (SR1-SR5) and Recovery night after the 5 sleep restriction nights (Rc). Baseline night (B) was the first night of the protocol followed either by acute sleep deprivation or chronic sleep restriction. Asterisks indicate significant differences with Baseline. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Squares indicate significant differences with acute sleep deprivation (only for the 5 sleep restriction nights). P < 0.01, P < 0.001.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Sleep latencies during the 6 naps on the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) across daytime after baseline night (A), acute sleep deprivation (B), second sleep restriction night (C), and fifth sleep restriction night (D).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean number of lapses (left) and mean Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) scores (right) fitted by the mixed-effects regression model. B, baseline; SR1-SR5, 5 consecutive sleep restriction nights.

Comment in

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Luckhaupt SE, Tak S, Calvert GM. The prevalence of short sleep duration by industry and occupation in the National Health Interview Survey. Sleep. 2010;33:149–59. - PMC - PubMed
    1. National Sleep Foundation. Sleep in America Poll: Summary of Findings. 2008. Available at http://www.sleepfoundation.org/sites/default/files/2008%20POLL%20SOF.PDF.
    1. Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26:117–26. - PubMed
    1. Belenky G, Wesensten NJ, Thorne DR, Thomas ML, Sing HC, Redmond DP, et al. Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. J Sleep Res. 2003;12:1–12. - PubMed
    1. Leproult R, Colecchia EF, Berardi AM, Stickgold R, Kosslyn SM, Van Cauter E. Individual differences in subjective and objective alertness during sleep deprivation are stable and unrelated. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2003;284:R280–90. - PubMed

Publication types