Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012;7(6):e38588.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038588. Epub 2012 Jun 26.

Modeling collective animal behavior with a cognitive perspective: a methodological framework

Affiliations

Modeling collective animal behavior with a cognitive perspective: a methodological framework

Sebastian Weitz et al. PLoS One. 2012.

Abstract

The last decades have seen an increasing interest in modeling collective animal behavior. Some studies try to reproduce as accurately as possible the collective dynamics and patterns observed in several animal groups with biologically plausible, individual behavioral rules. The objective is then essentially to demonstrate that the observed collective features may be the result of self-organizing processes involving quite simple individual behaviors. Other studies concentrate on the objective of establishing or enriching links between collective behavior researches and cognitive or physiological ones, which then requires that each individual rule be carefully validated. Here we discuss the methodological consequences of this additional requirement. Using the example of corpse clustering in ants, we first illustrate that it may be impossible to discriminate among alternative individual rules by considering only observational data collected at the group level. Six individual behavioral models are described: They are clearly distinct in terms of individual behaviors, they all reproduce satisfactorily the collective dynamics and distribution patterns observed in experiments, and we show theoretically that it is strictly impossible to discriminate two of these models even in the limit of an infinite amount of data whatever the accuracy level. A set of methodological steps are then listed and discussed as practical ways to partially overcome this problem. They involve complementary experimental protocols specifically designed to address the behavioral rules successively, conserving group-level data for the overall model validation. In this context, we highlight the importance of maintaining a sharp distinction between model enunciation, with explicit references to validated biological concepts, and formal translation of these concepts in terms of quantitative state variables and fittable functional dependences. Illustrative examples are provided of the benefits expected during the often long and difficult process of refining a behavioral model, designing adapted experimental protocols and inversing model parameters.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Clustering experiments.
Objects are uniformly distributed along the border of a 50 cm diameter circular arena. Ants enter the arena spontaneously from below, mounting along a wood stick through the hole in the arena center. Two different initial one-dimensional densities are used: 127 and 255 objects per meter. The duration of each experiment is 50 hours. Fig. A and Fig. B correspond respectively to the beginning and the end of a high density experiment. Fig. C and Fig. D display the time series of the number of piles (mean formula image s.d.) for the low and the high density experiments, respectively. Piles are defined as follows: Two neighboring objects are considered to belong to the same cluster if the distance between them is less than 1 mm. A cluster constitutes a pile if it contains at least 6 objects. In another circular arena with a 25 cm diameter was also used (with the same low and high initial densities). For the purpose of the methodological illustration in the first part of the results section only the large arena is used, but the model built in the second part of the results section is compatible with all observations, including the ones in small arenas (see Fig. 6).
Figure 2
Figure 2. Clustering dynamics predicted by the six different models.
Figs. A and B indicate the time series of the number of piles for models 1–6 compared to the experimental data (mean formula image s.d.) in the low and high density settings, respectively. The predictions of all the models are compatible with the experimental observations. Moreover, the predictions of model 5 are rigorously identical to those of model 3, and the predictions of model 6 are identical to those of model 4.
Figure 3
Figure 3. Definition of the object perception stimulus.
The grey area represents the objects distributed along the arena border and the dashed square the perception range of an ant at perimetric abscissa formula image and distance formula image to the border of the arena. The dashed grey area represents the fractions of the objects perceived by the ant (which corresponds to formula image in the text). When defining the perception stimulus at perimetric abscissa formula image, without knowing the distance to the border formula image (one-dimensional modeling), formula image is defined as the average of all values of formula image when formula image is uniformly distributed between formula image and the distance formula image of the external side of the farthest clustered object.
Figure 4
Figure 4. Parametric inversions for the direction change and deposition frequencies in the empty arena.
A) The survival curve of the proportion of ants still not having changed their direction (formula image trajectories) is compatible with an exponential fit formula image, validating the memoryless and instantaneous turn assumptions and leading to the direction change frequency formula image (mean formula image s.e.), formula image, formula image, formula image. B) The survival curve of the proportion of ants still not having deposited the object they loaded at time formula image in the empty arena (formula image trajectories) is also compatible with an exponential fit formula image and leads to the deposition frequency formula image, formula image, formula image, formula image. The black line represents the exponential fit and the dashed lines the 95% confidence interval. The formula image-values correspond to a chi-squared test for goodness of fit as described in (pp. 131–137).
Figure 5
Figure 5. Parametric inversion for the deposition frequency.
A) Functional dependence of the deposition frequency on the perception stimulus. B) Fit of the deposition frequency to the experimental deposition probabilities (mean formula image s.e.) on clusters of several sizes. The plain curve in A corresponds to the formal dependence retained in (formula image, with formula image and formula image, formula image, formula image, formula image). The dashed curve is a linear fit (formula image with formula image, formula image, formula image, formula image), very close to the previous formal dependence for small values of the perception stimulus. C) An alternative functional dependence of the deposition frequency on the perception stimulus, in which the deposition frequency is very low for small values of the perception stimulus and constant for values higher than a threshold corresponding to two objects entirely in the perception area. D) Adjustment of C to the experimental deposition probabilities (formula image if formula image and formula image else, with formula image, formula image and formula image, formula image, formula image, formula image). The grey circles correspond to the experimental data (mean formula image s.e.) and the formula image-values to a weighted least squares procedure.
Figure 6
Figure 6. Comparison with experimental data of the collective dynamics predicted by the finally retained model (that of the dashed curve in Figs. 5A and B).
Figs. A and B correspond to the experiments in the big arena, and Figs. C and D to those in the small arena, with low and high object densities, respectively (see Fig. 1). The grey + correspond to the experimental data (mean formula image s.d.) and the plain curves to the model predictions. The fitted number of ants in the arena is 50.
Figure 7
Figure 7. Significance of inter-individual variabilities and temporal correlations.
The survival probabilities formula image of models 5 or 6 (also to be interpreted as the population average of the survival probabilities of models 3 or 4) compared to an exponential fit corresponding to small formula image values (short time depositions). The extinction coefficient of the exponential is formula image.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Camazine S, Deneubourg JL, Franks NR, Sneyd J, Theraulaz G, et al. Self-organization in biological systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 2001.
    1. Couzin I, Krause J. Self-organization and collective behavior in vertebrates. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 2003;32:1–75.
    1. Sumpter D. Collective animal behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ Press. 2010.
    1. Ben-Jacob E. Bacterial self–organization: co–enhancement of complexification and adaptability in a dynamic environment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences. 2003;361:1283–1312. - PubMed
    1. Emlen Jr J. Flocking behavior in birds. The Auk. 1952;69:160–170.

Publication types