Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Jan 1;12(2-3):175-184.
doi: 10.1080/15295192.2012.683353. Epub 2012 Jun 14.

Differentiating Maternal Sensitivity to Infant Distress and Non-Distress

Affiliations

Differentiating Maternal Sensitivity to Infant Distress and Non-Distress

Esther M Leerkes et al. Parent Sci Pract. .

Abstract

Drawing from a domain specificity perspective, we assert that maternal sensitivity to infant distress cues is distinct from maternal sensitivity to non-distress cues. We review evidence from prior research demonstrating that the two constructs have more unshared than shared variance and that sensitivity to infant distress is a unique predictor of infants' early emotional well-being when both types of sensitivity are examined as simultaneous predictors. In addition, we present new evidence to test the hypothesis that maternal sensitivity to infant distress and non-distress have different origins. We draw on data from a subset of mothers and infants who participated in Phase I of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care (Study 1) and from 101 mother-infant dyads who participated in a longitudinal study of the origins of maternal sensitivity (Study 2). In both studies, maternal sensitivity to distress and non-distress were rated when infants were 6 months old. In both studies, socio-demographic risk (i.e., young, unmarried, low income mothers) was a stronger predictor of sensitivity to non-distress than of sensitivity to distress. In Study 2, mothers' emotional and cognitive responses to videotapes of crying infants during the prenatal period predicted maternal sensitivity during tasks designed to elicit infant fear and frustration but were unrelated to maternal sensitivity in a non-arousing free play context. Maternal sensitivity during infancy can be further divided into specific sub-types that have unique origins and unique effects on subsequent child well-being. Methodological, theoretical, and applied implications of such an approach are discussed.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Structural equation model predicting maternal sensitivity during distressing and non-distressing contexts with standardized estimates. *p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p <.001.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ainsworth MD, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978.
    1. Bornstein MH. Cultural approaches to parenting. Parenting: Science and Practice. 2012;12:XX–XX. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bornstein MH, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Maternal responsiveness and infant mental abilities: Specific predictive relations. Infant Behavior & Development. 1997;20:283–296.
    1. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. I. Attachment. NY: Basic Books; 1969/1982.
    1. Conger RD, Schofield TJ, Neppl TK. Intergenerational continuity and discontinuity in harsh parenting. Parenting: Science and Practice. 2012;12:XX–XX. - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources