Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Nov;83(6):1978-95.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01824.x. Epub 2012 Aug 2.

Infants use compression information to infer objects' weights: examining cognition, exploration, and prospective action in a preferential-reaching task

Affiliations

Infants use compression information to infer objects' weights: examining cognition, exploration, and prospective action in a preferential-reaching task

Petra Hauf et al. Child Dev. 2012 Nov.

Abstract

The present research used a preferential-reaching task to examine whether 9- and 11-month-olds (n=144) could infer the relative weights of two objects resting on a soft, compressible platform. Experiment 1 established that infants reached preferentially for the lighter of 2 boxes. In Experiments 2-4, infants saw 2 boxes identical except in weight resting on a cotton wool platform. Infants reached prospectively for the lighter box, but only when their initial exploratory activities provided critical information. At 11 months, infants succeeded as long as they first determined that the platform was compressible; at 9 months, infants succeeded only if they also explored the boxes and thus had advance knowledge that they differed in weight.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Schematic depiction of the stimuli and procedure used in Experiments 1 to 4.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean percentage of trials 9- and 11-month-olds reached preferentially for the lighter box in Experiments 1 to 4. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Correlations between time spent in weight-relevant exploration of the lighter and heavier boxes during the exploration phase (out of a maximum of 90 s, with 45 s per box) and reaching preferentially for the lighter box during the test phase. Correlations are shown separately for the 9- and 11-month-olds in Experiment 1 (top) and Experiment 2 (bottom).

References

    1. Adolph KE, Eppler MA, Marin L, Weise IB, Clearfield MW. Exploration in the service of prospective control. Infant Behavior and Development: Special Issue on Perception - Action Coupling. 2000;23:441–460.
    1. Adolph KE, Eppler MA, Gibson EJ. Development of perception of affordances. In: Rovee-Collier C, Lipsitt LP, editors. Advances in infancy research. Vol. 8. Ablex; Norwood, NJ: 1993. pp. 51–98.
    1. Aguiar A, Baillargeon R. Eight-and-a-half-month-old infants’ reasoning about containment events. Child Development. 1998;69:636–653. - PubMed
    1. Ahmed A, Ruffman T. Why do infants make A not B errors in a search task, yet show memory for the location of hidden objects in a non-search task? Developmental Psychology. 1998;34:441–453. - PubMed
    1. Baillargeon R. The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy: A summary in eight lessons. In: Goswami U, editor. Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. Blackwell; Oxford: 2002. pp. 47–83.

Publication types