Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Aug 1:3:258.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00258. eCollection 2012.

Face distortion aftereffects in personally familiar, famous, and unfamiliar faces

Affiliations

Face distortion aftereffects in personally familiar, famous, and unfamiliar faces

Billy Ronald Peter Walton et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

The internal face prototype is thought to be a construction of the average of every previously viewed face (Schwaninger et al., 2003). However, the influence of the most frequently encountered faces (i.e., personally familiar faces) has been generally understated. The current research explored the face distortion aftereffect in unfamiliar, famous, and personally familiar (each subject's parent) faces. Forty-eight adult participants reported whether faces were distorted or not (distorted by shifting the eyes in the vertical axis) of a series of images that included unfamiliar, famous, and personally familiar faces. The number of faces perceived to be "odd" was measured pre- and post-adaptation to the most extreme distortion. Participants were adapted to either an unfamiliar, famous, or personally familiar face. The results indicate that adaptation transferred from unfamiliar faces to personally familiar faces more so than the converse and aftereffects did not transfer from famous faces to unfamiliar faces. These results are indicative of representation differences between unfamiliar, famous, and personally familiar faces, whereby personally familiar faces share representations of both unfamiliar and famous faces.

Keywords: adaptation effects; face distortion aftereffects; face perception; personally familiar versus unfamiliar faces.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Running from left to right, a complete set of images for a negatively distorted parent including test images ranging from −1 through to −10 as well as the adaptor image of −25.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Magnitude of aftereffect (mean number of test faces perceived to be distorted post-adaptation subtracted from the mean number of faces perceived to be distorted at baseline), for each image-type for every adaptor type. Error bars show standard error.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Bahrick H. P., Bahrick P. O., Wittlinger R. P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for names and faces: a cross-sectional approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 104, 54–7510.1037/0096-3445.104.1.54 - DOI
    1. Bauer R. (1984). Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: a neuropsychological application of the guilty knowledge test. Neuropsychologia 22, 457–46910.1016/0028-3932(84)90040-X - DOI - PubMed
    1. Benton C., Jennings S., Chatting D. (2006). Viewpoint dependence in adaptation to facial identity. Vision Res. 46, 3313–332510.1016/j.visres.2006.06.002 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Blakemore C., Campbell F. (1969). On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 203, 237–260 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Blakemore C., Sutton P. (1969). Size adaptation: a new aftereffect. Science 166, 245–24710.1126/science.166.3902.245 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources