Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Sep 18;109(38):15191-6.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1213173109. Epub 2012 Sep 4.

Monkeys benefit from reciprocity without the cognitive burden

Affiliations

Monkeys benefit from reciprocity without the cognitive burden

Malini Suchak et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

The debate about the origins of human prosociality has focused on the presence or absence of similar tendencies in other species, and, recently, attention has turned to the underlying mechanisms. We investigated whether direct reciprocity could promote prosocial behavior in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Twelve capuchins tested in pairs could choose between two tokens, with one being "prosocial" in that it rewarded both individuals (i.e., 1/1), and the other being "selfish" in that it rewarded the chooser only (i.e., 1/0). Each monkey's choices with a familiar partner from their own group was compared with choices when paired with a partner from a different group. Capuchins were spontaneously prosocial, selecting the prosocial option at the same rate regardless of whether they were paired with an in-group or out-group partner. This indicates that interaction outside of the experimental setting played no role. When the paradigm was changed, such that both partners alternated making choices, prosocial preference significantly increased, leading to mutualistic payoffs. As no contingency could be detected between an individual's choice and their partner's previous choice, and choices occurred in rapid succession, reciprocity seemed of a relatively vague nature akin to mutualism. Having the partner receive a better reward than the chooser (i.e., 1/2) during the alternating condition increased the payoffs of mutual prosociality, and prosocial choice increased accordingly. The outcome of several controls made it hard to explain these results on the basis of reward distribution or learned preferences, and rather suggested that joint action promotes prosociality, resulting in so-called attitudinal reciprocity.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Experimental setup. A capuchin actor selects a token from the jumble of six tokens (n = 3 of each type). Her partner watches through the clear Lexan panel separating the two individuals. Following a token choice, the experimenter holds her hand up in a begging gesture, and the capuchin returns the token to her hand.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Comparison of prosocial choices across sessions. The mean (±SEM) percent prosocial choices across conditions for in-group and out-group pairs and partner-absent control sessions (*P < 0.05 and ***P < 0.001).
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
The effect of alternating and reward condition on prosocial choices. Although there was no significant difference between equal and unequal rewards, there was a significant interaction between equity and alternating choices (P < 0.05).

References

    1. Trivers RL. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol. 1971;46:35–57.
    1. Burkart JM, Hrdy SB, Van Schaik CP. Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evol Anthropol. 2009;18:175–186.
    1. de Waal FBM, Suchak M. Prosocial primates: Selfish and unselfish motivations. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010;365:2711–2722. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Batson CD, Moran T. Empathy-induced altruism in a prisoner’s dilemma. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1999;29:909–924.
    1. de Waal FBM. Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:279–300. - PubMed

Publication types