Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Sep 26:6:265.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00265. eCollection 2012.

Brain mechanisms underlying automatic and unconscious control of motor action

Affiliations

Brain mechanisms underlying automatic and unconscious control of motor action

Kevin D'Ostilio et al. Front Hum Neurosci. .

Abstract

Are we in command of our motor acts?The popular belief holds that our conscious decisions are the direct causes of our actions. However, overwhelming evidence from neurosciences demonstrates that our actions are instead largely driven by brain processes that unfold outside of our consciousness. To study these brain processes, scientists have used a range of different functional brain imaging techniques and experimental protocols, such as subliminal priming. Here, we review recent advances in the field and propose a theoretical model of motor control that may contribute to a better understanding of the pathophysiology of movement disorders such as Parkinson's disease.

Keywords: Parkinson disease; inhibition; motor control; neuroimaging; priming; subliminal; unconscious; volition.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Time of voluntary initiation of action and the associated brain regions. Frontal and parietal cortex work together for deciding the action to plan and are also involved in the awareness of motor intention whereas the SMA translates the first unconscious decision into movement. The subjective experience of will comes from the awareness of intention itself, the efferent signal from motor cortex to muscles and feedback sensorial signals.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Archibald S. J., Mateer C. A., Kerns K. A. (2001). Utilization behavior: clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychol. Rev. 11, 117–130 10.1023/A:1016673807158 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aron A. R., Schlaghecken F., Fletcher P. C., Bullmore E. T., Eimer M., Barker R., Sahakian B. J., Robbins T. W. (2003). Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is mediated by the caudate and thalamus: evidence from functional MRI and Huntington's disease. Brain 126, 713–723 10.1093/brain/awg067 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Aron A. R., Wise S. P., Poldrack R. A. (2009). Cognition: basal ganglia role, in The New Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, ed Squire L. R. (Oxford: Academic Press; ), 1069–1077
    1. Brasil-Neto J. P., Pascual-Leone A., Valls-Sole J., Cohen L. G., Hallett M. (1992). Focal transcranial magnetic stimulation and response bias in a forced-choice task. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 55, 964–966 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Dehaene S., Naccache L., Le Clec H. G., Koechlin E., Mueller M., Dehaene-Lambertz G., Van de Moortele P. F., Le Bihan D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature 395, 597–600 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1768 - DOI - PubMed