Automatic imitation in rhythmical actions: kinematic fidelity and the effects of compatibility, delay, and visual monitoring
- PMID: 23071623
- PMCID: PMC3465264
- DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046728
Automatic imitation in rhythmical actions: kinematic fidelity and the effects of compatibility, delay, and visual monitoring
Abstract
We demonstrate that observation of everyday rhythmical actions biases subsequent motor execution of the same and of different actions, using a paradigm where the observed actions were irrelevant for action execution. The cycle time of the distractor actions was subtly manipulated across trials, and the cycle time of motor responses served as the main dependent measure. Although distractor frequencies reliably biased response cycle times, this imitation bias was only a small fraction of the modulations in distractor speed, as well as of the modulations produced when participants intentionally imitated the observed rhythms. Importantly, this bias was not only present for compatible actions, but was also found, though numerically reduced, when distractor and executed actions were different (e.g., tooth brushing vs. window wiping), or when the dominant plane of movement was different (horizontal vs. vertical). In addition, these effects were equally pronounced for execution at 0, 4, and 8 s after action observation, a finding that contrasts with the more short-lived effects reported in earlier studies. The imitation bias was also unaffected when vision of the hand was occluded during execution, indicating that this effect most likely resulted from visuomotor interactions during distractor observation, rather than from visual monitoring and guidance during execution. Finally, when the distractor was incompatible in both dimensions (action type and plane) the imitation bias was not reduced further, in an additive way, relative to the single-incompatible conditions. This points to a mechanism whereby the observed action's impact on motor processing is generally reduced whenever this is not useful for motor planning. We interpret these findings in the framework of biased competition, where intended and distractor actions can be represented as competing and quasi-encapsulated sensorimotor streams.
Conflict of interest statement
Figures







Similar articles
-
A neural signature for combined action observation and motor imagery? An fNIRS study into prefrontal activation, automatic imitation, and self-other perceptions.Brain Behav. 2022 Feb;12(2):e2407. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2407. Epub 2022 Jan 7. Brain Behav. 2022. PMID: 34994997 Free PMC article.
-
Motor imagery during action observation modulates automatic imitation effects in rhythmical actions.Front Hum Neurosci. 2014 Feb 18;8:28. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00028. eCollection 2014. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014. PMID: 24600369 Free PMC article.
-
Motor imagery during action observation enhances imitation of everyday rhythmical actions in children with and without developmental coordination disorder.Hum Mov Sci. 2020 Jun;71:102620. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2020.102620. Epub 2020 Apr 15. Hum Mov Sci. 2020. PMID: 32452437
-
An active inference model of hierarchical action understanding, learning and imitation.Phys Life Rev. 2023 Sep;46:92-118. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2023.05.012. Epub 2023 Jun 5. Phys Life Rev. 2023. PMID: 37354642 Review.
-
Automatic imitation.Psychol Bull. 2011 May;137(3):463-83. doi: 10.1037/a0022288. Psychol Bull. 2011. PMID: 21280938 Review.
Cited by
-
When mirroring is both simple and "smart": how mimicry can be embodied, adaptive, and non-representational.Front Hum Neurosci. 2014 Jul 14;8:505. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00505. eCollection 2014. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014. PMID: 25071532 Free PMC article. Review.
-
A neural signature for combined action observation and motor imagery? An fNIRS study into prefrontal activation, automatic imitation, and self-other perceptions.Brain Behav. 2022 Feb;12(2):e2407. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2407. Epub 2022 Jan 7. Brain Behav. 2022. PMID: 34994997 Free PMC article.
-
Motor Imagery during Action Observation: A Brief Review of Evidence, Theory and Future Research Opportunities.Front Neurosci. 2016 Nov 21;10:514. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00514. eCollection 2016. Front Neurosci. 2016. PMID: 27917103 Free PMC article. Review.
-
Multiple roles of motor imagery during action observation.Front Hum Neurosci. 2013 Nov 25;7:807. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00807. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013. PMID: 24324428 Free PMC article. Review.
-
Enhancing motor imagery practice using synchronous action observation.Psychol Res. 2024 Sep;88(6):1891-1907. doi: 10.1007/s00426-022-01768-7. Epub 2022 Dec 27. Psychol Res. 2024. PMID: 36574019 Free PMC article. Review.
References
-
- Chartrand TL, van Baaren R (2009) Human mimicry. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 41: 219–274.
-
- Vogt S, Thomaschke R (2007) From visuomotor interactions to imitation learning: Behavioural and brain imaging studies. J Sports Sci 25: 497–517. - PubMed
-
- Heyes CM (2011) Automatic imitation. Psychol Bull 137: 463–483. - PubMed
-
- Brass M, Bekkering H, Wohlschlager A, Prinz W (2000) Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain Cogn 44: 124–143. - PubMed
-
- Brass M, Bekkering H, Prinz W (2001) Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychol 106: 3–22. - PubMed
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources