Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2011;2(5):440-57.
doi: 10.1068/i0406. Epub 2011 Aug 8.

Sensitivity to first- and second-order drifting gratings in 3-month-old infants

Affiliations

Sensitivity to first- and second-order drifting gratings in 3-month-old infants

Vickie Armstrong et al. Iperception. 2011.

Abstract

In two experiments, we investigated 3-month-old infants' sensitivity to first- and second-order drifting gratings. In Experiment 1 we used forced-choice preferential looking with drifting versus stationary gratings to estimate depth modulation thresholds for 3-month-old infants and a similar task for a comparison group of adults. Thresholds for infants were more adult-like for second-order than first-order gratings. In Experiment 2, 3-month-olds dishabituated to a change in first-order orientation, but not to a change in direction of first- or second-order motion. Hence, results from Experiment 1 were likely driven by the perception of flicker rather than motion. Thus, infants' sensitivity to uniform motion is slow to develop and appears to be driven initially by flicker-sensitive mechanisms. The underlying mechanisms have more mature tuning for second-order than for first-order information.

Keywords: Development; first-order; flicker; infants; motion; second-order.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Experimeter's accuracy (mean percent of correct responses (±1 s.e) based on 3-month-olds' responses to moving versus stationary (a) first-order gratings and (b) second-order gratings. The 50% value on the y-axis represents performance expected by chance. Values marked with an asterisk indicate values that are significantly above chance (p < .0.125). Note that the values on the x-axes (log luminance- or contrast-modulation) cannot be compared directly because they are in units relevant to two different properties (change in luminance versus in contrast, respectively).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
infants' estimated log thresholds and adults' mean log thresholds for luminance-modulated (LM) and contrast-modulated (CM) stimuli. Error bars denote the estimated range of the threshold for infants and standard error of the mean for adults. On a log scale, negative values represent modulations lower than 1%.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Three-month-olds' looking times (±1 s.e) during the test phase after habituation to vertical gratings moving leftward or rightward. Results are for vertical gratings moving in the habituated direction, vertical gratings moving in a novel opposite direction, and horizontal gratings moving downward (both a novel orientation and direction of motion) for (a) first-order and (b) second-order motion. infants' looking times did not differ for the novel versus habituated direction of motion. However, infants did look significantly longer at the grating with novel orientation than the habituated orientation, but only when tested with first-order stimuli.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Allen H A, Ledgeway T, Hess R F. “Poor encoding of position by contrast-defined motion”. Vision Research. 2004;44:17–17. - PubMed
    1. Armstrong V, Lewis T L, Maurer D. “Sensitivity to first- and second-order motion and form in children and adults”. Vision Research. 2009;49:2774–2781. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.016. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ashida H, Lingnau A, Wall M B, Smith A T. “FMRI adaptation reveals separate mechanisms for first-order and second-order motion”. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97:1319–1325. doi: 10.1152/jn.00723.2006. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aslin R N, Shea S L. “Velocity thresholds in human infants: Implications for the perception of motion Developmental Psychology”. 2. 1990;6:589–598.
    1. Atkinson J, Braddick O. “Development of optokinetic nystagmus in infants: An indicator of cortical binocularity?”. In: Fisher D F, A Monty R, W Senders J, editors. Eye movements: Cognition and Visual Perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1981. pp. 53–64.

LinkOut - more resources