Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012;7(11):e48961.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048961. Epub 2012 Nov 14.

Cognitive biases toward Internet game-related pictures and executive deficits in individuals with an Internet game addiction

Affiliations

Cognitive biases toward Internet game-related pictures and executive deficits in individuals with an Internet game addiction

Zhenhe Zhou et al. PLoS One. 2012.

Abstract

Background: The cue-related go/no-go switching task provides an experimental approach to study individual's flexibility in changing situations. Because Internet addiction disorder (IAD) belongs to the compulsive-impulsive spectrum of disorders, it should present cognitive bias and executive functioning deficit characteristics of some of these types of disorders. Until now, no studies have been reported on cognitive bias and executive function involving mental flexibility and response inhibition in IAD.

Methodology/principal findings: A total of 46 subjects who met the criteria of the modified Young's Diagnostic Questionnaire for Internet addiction (YDQ) were recruited as an Internet game addiction (IGA) group, along with 46 healthy control individuals. All participants performed the Internet game-shifting task. Using hit rate, RT, d' and C as the dependent measures, a three-way ANOVA (group × target × condition) was performed. For hit rate, a significant effect of group, type of target and condition were found. The group-target interaction effect was significant. For RT, significant effects were revealed for group and type of target. The group-target interaction effect was significant. Comparisons of the means revealed that the slowing down of IGA relative to NIA was more pronounced when the target stimuli were neutral as opposed to Internet game-related pictures. In addition, the group-condition interaction effect was significant. For d', significant effects of group, type of target and condition were found. The group-target interaction effect was significant. For C, the type of target produced a significant effect. There was a positive correlation between the length of the addiction (number of years) and the severity of the cognitive bias.

Conclusions: IGA present cognitive biases towards information related to Internet gaming. These biases, as well as poor executive functioning skills (lower mental flexibility and response inhibition), might be responsible for Internet game addiction. The assessment of cognitive biases in IGA might provide a methodology for evaluation of therapeutic effects.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Internet game-shifting task.
Figure 2
Figure 2. The switch effect and target effect value and standard-error bars for hit rate between IGA (white) and NIA (black) group on the Internet game-shifting task.
Figure 3
Figure 3. The switch effect and target effect value and standard-error bars for RTs between IGA (white) and NIA (black) group on the Internet game-shifting task.
Figure 4
Figure 4. The switch effect and target effect value and standard-error bars for d′ between IGA (white) and NIA (black) group on the Internet game-shifting task.
Figure 5
Figure 5. The switch effect and target effect value and standard-error bars for C between IGA (white) and NIA (black) group on the Internet game-shifting task.
Figure 6
Figure 6. Relationship between years of addiction and cognitive measures in IGA.

References

    1. Lauretti WJ, Brady D (1996) Chiropractic and the information superhighway: a brief guide to the Internet. Journal of manipulative and physiological therapeutics19 (4): 273–277. - PubMed
    1. Cheung LM, Wong WS (2011) The effects of insomnia and internet addiction on depression in Hong Kong Chinese adolescents: an exploratory cross-sectional analysis. Journal of sleep research 20(2): 311–317. - PubMed
    1. Byun S, Ruffini C, Mills JE, Douglas AC, Niang M, et al. (2009) Internet addiction: meta-synthesis of 1996–2006 quantitative research. CyberPsychology and Behavior 12(2): 203–207. - PubMed
    1. Davis RA (2001) A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological Internet use. Computers in Human Behavior 17: 187–195.
    1. Young KS, Rogers RC (1998) The relationship between depression and Internet addiction. Computers in Human Behavior 1: 25–128.

Publication types