Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Jan 16;280(1754):20122830.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2830. Print 2013 Mar 7.

Perception of silent and motionless prey on vegetation by echolocation in the gleaning bat Micronycteris microtis

Affiliations

Perception of silent and motionless prey on vegetation by echolocation in the gleaning bat Micronycteris microtis

Inga Geipel et al. Proc Biol Sci. .

Abstract

Gleaning insectivorous bats that forage by using echolocation within dense forest vegetation face the sensorial challenge of acoustic masking effects. Active perception of silent and motionless prey in acoustically cluttered environments by echolocation alone has thus been regarded impossible. The gleaning insectivorous bat Micronycteris microtis however, forages in dense understory vegetation and preys on insects, including dragonflies, which rest silent and motionless on vegetation. From behavioural experiments, we show that M. microtis uses echolocation as the sole sensorial modality for successful prey perception within a complex acoustic environment. All individuals performed a stereotypical three-dimensional hovering flight in front of prey items, while continuously emitting short, multi-harmonic, broadband echolocation calls. We observed a high precision in target localization which suggests that M. microtis perceives a detailed acoustic image of the prey based on shape, surface structure and material. Our experiments provide, to our knowledge, the first evidence that a gleaning bat uses echolocation alone for successful detection, classification and precise localization of silent and motionless prey in acoustic clutter. Overall, we conclude that the three-dimensional hovering flight of M. microtis in combination with a frequent emission of short, high-frequency echolocation calls is the key for active prey perception in acoustically highly cluttered environments.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Experimental flight cage (1.40 × 1.00 × 0.80 m) viewed from the top with potted control and experimental plant. Target presentation on experimental plant (e.g. complete dragonfly).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Picture series gained from a high-speed infrared video recording (EMS) of M. microtis scanning and acquiring target (complete dragonfly): white arrows indicate the subsequent direction of the individual's movement. Small white cross marks the back of the bat's head. (a) Beginning of the scanning behaviour with the bat moving upwards, to the right-hand side of the leaf. (b) Movement from the right-hand side (distance to prey ca 10 cm) of the leaf downwards. (c) Movement towards the centre of the leaf with the dragonfly (distance ca 14 cm). (d) The bat flies closer towards the leaf while moving slightly upwards (from a distance of ca 8 cm to a distance of ca 5 cm to prey). Bat hovering on the spot close to prey. (e) With the head directed towards the prey, the bat briefly flies backwards (approx. 5 cm). (f) Bat changes flight direction again and moves forward with its head turned slightly upwards (distance approx. 9 cm). (g) Final approach. (h) End of scanning by touching the experimental leaf and landing on the prey. Taking prey off the leaf. (i) Take off with the dragonfly.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Number of landings on the seven presented targets versus prey rejections of seven M. microtis individuals.

Comment in

Similar articles

Cited by

  • The aerodynamic cost of head morphology in bats: maybe not as bad as it seems.
    Vanderelst D, Peremans H, Razak NA, Verstraelen E, Dimitriadis G. Vanderelst D, et al. PLoS One. 2015 Mar 4;10(3):e0118545. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118545. eCollection 2015. PLoS One. 2015. PMID: 25739038 Free PMC article.
  • Biosonar resolving power: echo-acoustic perception of surface structures in the submillimeter range.
    Simon R, Knörnschild M, Tschapka M, Schneider A, Passauer N, Kalko EK, von Helversen O. Simon R, et al. Front Physiol. 2014 Feb 26;5:64. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2014.00064. eCollection 2014. Front Physiol. 2014. PMID: 24616703 Free PMC article.
  • The Sonozotz project: Assembling an echolocation call library for bats in a megadiverse country.
    Zamora-Gutierrez V, Ortega J, Avila-Flores R, Aguilar-Rodríguez PA, Alarcón-Montano M, Avila-Torresagatón LG, Ayala-Berdón J, Bolívar-Cimé B, Briones-Salas M, Chan-Noh M, Chávez-Cauich M, Chávez C, Cortés-Calva P, Cruzado J, Cuevas JC, Del Real-Monroy M, Elizalde-Arellano C, García-Luis M, García-Morales R, Guerrero JA, Guevara-Carrizales AA, Gutiérrez EG, Hernández-Mijangos LA, Ibarra-López MP, Iñiguez-Dávalos LI, León-Madrazo R, López-González C, López-Téllez MC, López-Vidal JC, Martínez-Balvanera S, Montiel-Reyes F, Murrieta-Galindo R, Orozco-Lugo CL, Pech-Canché JM, Pérez-Pérez L, Ramírez-Martínez MM, Rizo-Aguilar A, Robredo-Esquivelzeta E, Rodas-Martínez AZ, Rojo-Cruz MA, Selem-Salas CI, Uribe-Bencomo E, Vargas-Contreras JA, MacSwiney G MC. Zamora-Gutierrez V, et al. Ecol Evol. 2020 May 18;10(11):4928-4943. doi: 10.1002/ece3.6245. eCollection 2020 Jun. Ecol Evol. 2020. PMID: 32551071 Free PMC article.
  • Ear pinnae in a neotropical katydid (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) function as ultrasound guides for bat detection.
    Pulver CA, Celiker E, Woodrow C, Geipel I, Soulsbury CD, Cullen DA, Rogers SM, Veitch D, Montealegre-Z F. Pulver CA, et al. Elife. 2022 Sep 28;11:e77628. doi: 10.7554/eLife.77628. Elife. 2022. PMID: 36170144 Free PMC article.
  • Airport noise disturbs foraging behavior of Japanese pipistrelle bats.
    Wang W, Gao H, Li C, Deng Y, Zhou D, Li Y, Zhou W, Luo B, Liang H, Liu W, Wu P, Jing W, Feng J. Wang W, et al. Ecol Evol. 2022 Jun 12;12(6):e8976. doi: 10.1002/ece3.8976. eCollection 2022 Jul. Ecol Evol. 2022. PMID: 35784026 Free PMC article.

References

    1. Griffin DR. 1986. Listening in the dark, 2nd edn Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press
    1. Johnson M, Madsen PT, Zimmer WMX, de Soto NA, Tyack PL. 2004. Beaked whales echolocate on prey. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, S383–S38610.1098/rsbl.2004.0208 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0208) - DOI - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Au WWL. 1993. The sonar of dolphins. New York, NY: Springer
    1. Novick A. 1959. Acoustic orientation in the cave swiftlet. Biol. Bull. 117, 497–50310.1103/PhysRevE.66.035103 (doi:10.1103/PhysRevE.66.035103) - DOI - DOI
    1. Konishi M, Knudsen EI. 1979. The oilbird: hearing and echolocation. Science 204, 425–42710.1126/science.441731 (doi:10.1126/science.441731) - DOI - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources