Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Jan 30;280(1755):20122654.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2654. Print 2013 Mar 22.

Early false-belief understanding in traditional non-Western societies

Affiliations

Early false-belief understanding in traditional non-Western societies

H Clark Barrett et al. Proc Biol Sci. .

Abstract

The psychological capacity to recognize that others may hold and act on false beliefs has been proposed to reflect an evolved, species-typical adaptation for social reasoning in humans; however, controversy surrounds the developmental timing and universality of this trait. Cross-cultural studies using elicited-response tasks indicate that the age at which children begin to understand false beliefs ranges from 4 to 7 years across societies, whereas studies using spontaneous-response tasks with Western children indicate that false-belief understanding emerges much earlier, consistent with the hypothesis that false-belief understanding is a psychological adaptation that is universally present in early childhood. To evaluate this hypothesis, we used three spontaneous-response tasks that have revealed early false-belief understanding in the West to test young children in three traditional, non-Western societies: Salar (China), Shuar/Colono (Ecuador) and Yasawan (Fiji). Results were comparable with those from the West, supporting the hypothesis that false-belief understanding reflects an adaptation that is universally present early in development.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Maps showing the locations of the Salar (China), Shuar/Colono (Ecuador), and Yasawan (Fiji) field sites. (Online version in colour.)
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Verbal preferential-looking false-belief task. (a) Set-up trials (striped bars represent matching picture and unstriped bars represent non-matching picture) and (b) test trial (striped bars represent initial-location picture and unstriped bars represent current-location picture). Bars show mean looking times, error bars show standard errors of the mean, and asterisks denote significant differences between two bars (p<0.05, two-tailed).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Verbal anticipatory-looking false-belief task. Bars show mean looking times, error bars show standard errors of the mean, and asterisks denote significant differences between two bars (p<0.05, two-tailed). Striped bars represent target container and unstriped bars represent non-target container.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Non-verbal violation-of-expectation false-belief task. Bars show mean looking times, error bars show standard errors of the mean, and asterisks denote significant differences between two bars (p<0.05, two-tailed). Striped bars represent different-object event and unstriped bars represent identical-object event.

References

    1. Dally JM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS. 2006. Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when. Science 312, 1662–166510.1126/science.1126539 (doi:10.1126/science.1126539) - DOI - DOI - PubMed
    1. Call J, Tomasello M. 2008. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 187–19210.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010 (doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010) - DOI - DOI - PubMed
    1. Marticorena DCW, Ruiz AM, Mukerji C, Goddu A, Santos LR. 2011. Monkeys represent others’ knowledge but not their beliefs. Dev. Sci. 14, 1406–141610.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01085.x (doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01085.x) - DOI - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Baillargeon R, He Z, Setoh P, Scott RM, Sloane S, Yang Y. In press. False-belief understanding and why it matters: the social-acting hypothesis. In Navigating the social world: what infants, children, and other species can teach us (eds Banaji MR, Gelman S.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press
    1. Sperber D, Wilson D. 1995. Relevance: communication and cognition, 2nd edn Oxford, UK: Blackwell

Publication types