Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Clinical Trial
. 2013 Mar 12;110(11):4357-62.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1213555110. Epub 2013 Feb 11.

Reversal of age-related neural timing delays with training

Affiliations
Clinical Trial

Reversal of age-related neural timing delays with training

Samira Anderson et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

Neural slowing is commonly noted in older adults, with consequences for sensory, motor, and cognitive domains. One of the deleterious effects of neural slowing is impairment of temporal resolution; older adults, therefore, have reduced ability to process the rapid events that characterize speech, especially in noisy environments. Although hearing aids provide increased audibility, they cannot compensate for deficits in auditory temporal processing. Auditory training may provide a strategy to address these deficits. To that end, we evaluated the effects of auditory-based cognitive training on the temporal precision of subcortical processing of speech in noise. After training, older adults exhibited faster neural timing and experienced gains in memory, speed of processing, and speech-in-noise perception, whereas a matched control group showed no changes. Training was also associated with decreased variability of brainstem response peaks, suggesting a decrease in temporal jitter in response to a speech signal. These results demonstrate that auditory-based cognitive training can partially restore age-related deficits in temporal processing in the brain; this plasticity in turn promotes better cognitive and perceptual skills.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Flow of participants randomly assigned to auditory training or active control groups.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
The stimulus waveform [da] (gray) and the grand average response waveform to the [da] presented in quiet (n = 67) at pretest (black).
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Changes in the neural response to [da] for peaks occurring every 10 ms (corresponding to the 100 Hz pitch of the stimulus) are displayed for the auditory training (red; n = 35) and active control (blue; n = 32) groups in quiet (Upper) and noise (Lower). The brackets indicate the transition and steady-state regions of the [da]. Improvements in timing were noted in the auditory training but not in the active control group. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001; Error bars: ±1 SE.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
Individual average timing changes for the formant transition (Left) and steady-state vowel (Right) in the auditory training (Upper) and active control (Lower) groups for neural responses to speech in noise. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001.
Fig. 5.
Fig. 5.
Quiet-to-noise timing shift differences. Responses in quiet and noise were cross-correlated to obtain an objective measure of timing (lag). (A) Before training, the expected noise-induced timing shift is observed, with the response in noise (black) lagging behind the response in quiet (gray) by 0.2–0.3 ms (shown in an individual participant). (B) After training, this individual’s response in noise (red) now overlays the response in quiet (pink) with minimal apparent lag. (C) Lag changes in each participant in the auditory training group, indicating a significant group change from pre- to posttest. (D) Mean group lag changes. *P < 0.01; error bars: ±1 SE.
Fig. 6.
Fig. 6.
Improvements in speech-in-noise perception, short-term memory, and processing speed were only observed in the auditory training group. **P < 0.01 group × test interactions; error bars: ±1 SE.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Lu PH, et al. Age-related slowing in cognitive processing speed is associated with myelin integrity in a very healthy elderly sample. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2011;33(10):1059–1068. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Parthasarathy A, Bartlett EL. Age-related auditory deficits in temporal processing in F-344 rats. Neuroscience. 2011;192(0):619–630. - PubMed
    1. Recanzone GH, Engle JR, Juarez-Salinas DL. Spatial and temporal processing of single auditory cortical neurons and populations of neurons in the macaque monkey. Hear Res. 2011;271(1-2):115–122. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Walton JP, Frisina RD, O’Neill WE. Age-related alteration in processing of temporal sound features in the auditory midbrain of the CBA mouse. J Neurosci. 1998;18(7):2764–2776. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Forstmann BU, et al. The speed-accuracy tradeoff in the elderly brain: A structural model-based approach. J Neurosci. 2011;31(47):17242–17249. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types