Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Feb 12:4:58.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00058. eCollection 2013.

Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is

Affiliations

Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is

Andrew D Wilson et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

The most exciting hypothesis in cognitive science right now is the theory that cognition is embodied. Like all good ideas in cognitive science, however, embodiment immediately came to mean six different things. The most common definitions involve the straight-forward claim that "states of the body modify states of the mind." However, the implications of embodiment are actually much more radical than this. If cognition can span the brain, body, and the environment, then the "states of mind" of disembodied cognitive science won't exist to be modified. Cognition will instead be an extended system assembled from a broad array of resources. Taking embodiment seriously therefore requires both new methods and theory. Here we outline four key steps that research programs should follow in order to fully engage with the implications of embodiment. The first step is to conduct a task analysis, which characterizes from a first person perspective the specific task that a perceiving-acting cognitive agent is faced with. The second step is to identify the task-relevant resources the agent has access to in order to solve the task. These resources can span brain, body, and environment. The third step is to identify how the agent can assemble these resources into a system capable of solving the problem at hand. The last step is to test the agent's performance to confirm that agent is actually using the solution identified in step 3. We explore these steps in more detail with reference to two useful examples (the outfielder problem and the A-not-B error), and introduce how to apply this analysis to the thorny question of language use. Embodied cognition is more than we think it is, and we have the tools we need to realize its full potential.

Keywords: A-not-B error; dynamical systems; embodied cognition; language; outfielder problem; replacement hypothesis; robotics.

PubMed Disclaimer

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adam H., Galinsky A. (2012). Enclothed cognition. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 918–92510.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.008 - DOI
    1. Baillargeon R., Graber M. (1988). Evidence of a location memory in 8-month old infants in a non-search A-not-B task. Dev. Psychol. 24, 502–51110.1037/0012-1649.24.4.502 - DOI
    1. Ballerini M., Cabibbo N., Candelier R., Cavagna A., Cisbani E., Giardina I., et al. (2007). Interaction ruling animal collective behaviour depends on topological rather than metric distance: evidence from a field study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 1232–123710.1073/pnas.0711437105 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Barrett L. (2011). Beyond the Brain: How the Body and the Environment Shape Cognition. New Jersey: Princeton University Press
    1. Barsalou L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 577–66010.1017/S0140525X99002149 - DOI - PubMed