Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Mar;133(3):EL174-80.
doi: 10.1121/1.4789864.

Accent-independent adaptation to foreign accented speech

Affiliations

Accent-independent adaptation to foreign accented speech

Melissa M Baese-Berk et al. J Acoust Soc Am. 2013 Mar.

Abstract

Foreign-accented speech can be difficult to understand but listeners can adapt to novel talkers and accents with appropriate experience. Previous studies have demonstrated talker-independent but accent-dependent learning after training on multiple talkers from a single language background. Here, listeners instead were exposed to talkers from five language backgrounds during training. After training, listeners generalized their learning to novel talkers from language backgrounds both included and not included in the training set. These findings suggest that generalization of foreign-accent adaptation is the result of exposure to systematic variability in accented speech that is similar across talkers from multiple language backgrounds.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Performance (proportion of keywords correct) on the post-test for a novel talker with a novel accent (Slovakian-accented English). Participants were trained on native English talkers (“No Foreign Accent”), five Mandarin-accented talkers (“Single Foreign Accent”), or five talkers from different native language backgrounds (“Multiple Foreign Accent”). Only participants in the Multiple-Foreign-Accent training group demonstrate generalization to a novel talker from a novel accent background.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Performance (proportion of keywords correct) on the post-test for a novel talker with a familiar accent (Mandarin-accented English). Otherwise as in Fig. 1. Participants in both the Single-Foreign-Accent and Multiple-Foreign-Accent groups demonstrate generalization to a novel talker from a familiar accent background.

References

    1. Baker, R. E. (2010). “The acquisition of English focus marking by non-native speakers,” Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.
    1. Baker, R. E., Bonnasse-Gahot, L., Kim, M., Van Engen, K. J., and Bradlow, A. R. (2011). “Word durations in non-native English,” J. Phonetics 39(1), 1–17.10.1016/j.wocn.2010.10.006 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bamford, J., and Wilson, I. (1979). “Methodological considerations and practical aspects of the BKB sentence lists,” in Speech-Hearing Tests and the Spoken Language of Hearing-Impaired Children, edited by Bench J. and Bamford J. (Academic Press, London).
    1. Bates, D. M., and Macheler, M. (2009). lme4: Liner mixed-effects models using S4 classes, R package version 0.999375-42.
    1. Bench, J., and Bamford, J., editors. (1979). Speech-Hearing Tests and the Spoken Language of Hearing-Impaired Children (Academic Press, London).

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources