Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Apr 10;13(5):8.
doi: 10.1167/13.5.8.

Independence is elusive: set size effects on encoding precision in visual search

Affiliations

Independence is elusive: set size effects on encoding precision in visual search

Helga Mazyar et al. J Vis. .

Abstract

Looking for a target in a visual scene becomes more difficult as the number of stimuli increases. In a signal detection theory view, this is due to the cumulative effect of noise in the encoding of the distractors, and potentially on top of that, to an increase of the noise (i.e., a decrease of precision) per stimulus with set size, reflecting divided attention. It has long been argued that human visual search behavior can be accounted for by the first factor alone. While such an account seems to be adequate for search tasks in which all distractors have the same, known feature value (i.e., are maximally predictable), we recently found a clear effect of set size on encoding precision when distractors are drawn from a uniform distribution (i.e., when they are maximally unpredictable). Here we interpolate between these two extreme cases to examine which of both conclusions holds more generally as distractor statistics are varied. In one experiment, we vary the level of distractor heterogeneity; in another we dissociate distractor homogeneity from predictability. In all conditions in both experiments, we found a strong decrease of precision with increasing set size, suggesting that precision being independent of set size is the exception rather than the rule.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
How does precision depend on set size? Each box in the diagram represents a distractor condition in single-target visual search. The gray boxes represent conditions for which set size effects on precision have been studied previously: homogeneous distractors that are predictable on every trial (bottom left) and maximally heterogeneous distractors that are also maximally unpredictable (top right). In this paper, we interpolate between these conditions (Experiment 1) and dissociate the factors (Experiment 2).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Experiment 1. (a) Time course of a single trial. (b) Von Mises distributions from which the distractor orientations were drawn in the three conditions (low, medium, and high heterogeneity). (c) Sample search displays in the three conditions. Displays are not to scale.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Data from Experiment 1. Here and elsewhere, error bars indicate 1 SEM. (a) Mean performance for the three heterogeneity conditions. Each condition was presented in two out of six sessions. (b) Hit and false-alarm rates as a function of set size for each condition.
Figure 4
Figure 4
VPO model fits in Experiment 1. (a) Hit and false-alarm rates in the three heterogeneity conditions. Here and elsewhere, shaded areas indicate 1 SEM in the model, and numbers indicate the root-mean-square error between model and data, averaged over subjects. (b) Proportion “target present” responses as a function of the minimum target-distractor difference, separately for target-present (blue) and target-absent (red) trials. For set size 1, there are no distractors on target-present trials. Note that the values on the x-axes differ between rows.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Model comparison results for Experiment 1. Shown are BIC values of the EPO, EPM, and VPM models relative to the VPO model, for each subject (left) as well as the group averages (right). Each row corresponds to a heterogeneity condition. Higher BIC values mean worse fits.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Dependence of precision on set size in Experiment 1. Estimates of mean precision parameter at each set size in the VPO (black) and VPM (red) models. The right y-axis shows the corresponding standard deviation of the Gaussian noise distributions, computed using the mapping σ2 = 1/(4) (see Models). The shades represent the best-fitting power law (mean over subjects; width indicates 1 SEM).
Figure 7
Figure 7
Experiment 2. (a) Time course of a trial (heterogeneous condition). Distractor orientations were drawn from a Von Mises distribution with a concentration parameter κD = 32.8 (corresponding to 5°; inset). Subjects judged whether a vertical target was present among the stimuli. (b) In the heterogeneous condition, distractor orientations were drawn independently. In the homogeneous condition, a common distractor orientation was drawn on each trial and assigned to all distractors on that trial.
Figure 8
Figure 8
Data and VPO model fits from Experiment 2. Top row: homogeneous; bottom row: heterogeneous. (a) Hit and false alarm: data (circles and error bars) and VPO model fits (shaded areas). (b) Proportion “target present” responses as a function of the target-distractor difference, separately for target-present (blue) and target-absent (red) trials. For set size 1, there are no distractors on target-present trials. Note that the values on the x-axes and the set sizes are different in the homogeneous and heterogeneous conditions.
Figure 9
Figure 9
Comparison between conditions of the relationship between mean precision and set size in the VPO model. (a) Mean precision estimates for the heterogeneous (black) and homogeneous (red) conditions of Experiment 2. (b) Estimates of the power in the relationship between mean precision and set size for all conditions in Experiments 1 and 2.
Figure A1
Figure A1
Generative models. These diagrams depict the dependencies between the variable of interest (target presence, C) and the measurements (x). Part of the statistical structure is shared (top), and part of it is specific to the experiment (bottom). 0 is the zero vector, and 1i is a vector of zeroes with a 1 in the ith entry. VM stands for the Von Mises distribution on (−90°, 90°); in parentheses are its argument, its mean, and its concentration parameter.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Baldassi S., Burr D. C. (2000). Feature-based integration of orientation signals in visual search. Vision Research, 40, 1293–1300 - PubMed
    1. Bauer B., Jolicoeur P., Cowan W. B. (1996). Visual search for colour targets that are or are not linearly separable from distractors. Vision Research, 36 (101), 1439–1465 - PubMed
    1. Beck J. M., Ma W. J., Pitkow X., Latham P. E., Pouget A. (2012). Not noisy, just wrong: The role of suboptimal inference in behavioral variability. Neuron, 74 (1), 30–39 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Broadbent D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon;
    1. Busey T., Palmer J. (2008). Set-size effects for identification versus localization depend on the visual search task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 34 (4), 790–810 - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources