Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2013 Apr 15;13(3):7.
doi: 10.1167/13.3.7.

A value-driven mechanism of attentional selection

Affiliations
Review

A value-driven mechanism of attentional selection

Brian A Anderson. J Vis. .

Abstract

Attention selects stimuli for cognitive processing, and the mechanisms that underlie the process of attentional selection have been a major topic of psychological research for over 30 years. From this research, it has been well documented that attentional selection can proceed both voluntarily, driven by visual search goals, and involuntarily, driven by the physical salience of stimuli. In this review, I provide a conceptual framework for attentional control that emphasizes the need for stimulus selection to promote the survival and wellbeing of an organism. I argue that although goal-driven and salience-driven mechanisms of attentional selection fit within this framework, a central component that is missing is a mechanism of attentional selection that is uniquely driven by learned associations between stimuli and rewards. I go on to review recent evidence for such a value-driven mechanism of attentional selection, and describe how this mechanism functions independently of the well-documented salience-driven and goal-driven mechanisms. I conclude by arguing that reward learning modifies the attentional priority of stimuli, allowing them to compete more effectively for selection even when nonsalient and task-irrelevant.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Example displays from the training phase and test phase of paradigms used to assess value-driven attention capture. (A) Training phase with reward feedback. (B) Test phase with nonsalient distractors. (C) Test phase involving a different task. (D) Test phase with physically salient distractors. (E) Training phase without reward feedback. Each trial was followed by a blank intertrial interval. The number of trials, the magnitude of reward, the duration of each display, and the position of the colored stimuli varied across different experiments. ISI: interstimulus interval.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Value-driven attentional capture, reflected in the slowing of response time by the presence of previously reward-associated distractors. (A) Results from experiment 1 of Anderson et al. (2011b), following 1,008 trials of training. (B) Results from experiment 3 of Anderson et al. (2011b), following 240 trials of training. (C) Results from experiment 1 of Anderson and Yantis (2012), following 300 trials of training. (D) Results from Anderson and Yantis (2013), following participation in A–C.

References

    1. Anderson B. A., Folk C. L. (2010). Variations in the magnitude of attentional capture: Testing a two-process model. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 342–352 - PubMed
    1. Anderson B. A., Folk C. L. (2012). Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1183–1198 - PubMed
    1. Anderson B. A., Laurent P. A., Yantis S. (2011a). Learned value magnifies salience-based attentional capture. PLoS One, 6, e27926 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson B. A., Laurent P. A., Yantis S. (2011b). Value-driven attentional capture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108, 10367–10371 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson B. A., Laurent P. A., Yantis S. (2012). Generalization of value-based attentional priority. Visual Cognition, 20, 647–658 - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources