Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 May 8:4:98.
doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00098. eCollection 2013.

Echolocation may have real-life advantages for blind people: an analysis of survey data

Affiliations

Echolocation may have real-life advantages for blind people: an analysis of survey data

Lore Thaler. Front Physiol. .

Abstract

Some people can echolocate by making sonar emissions (e.g., mouth-clicks, finger snaps, feet shuffling, humming, cane tapping, etc.) and listening to the returning echoes. To date there are no statistics available about how many blind people use echolocation, but anecdotal reports in the literature suggest that perhaps between 20 and 30% of totally blind people may use it, suggesting that echolocation affords broad functional benefits. Consistent with the notion that blind individuals benefit from the use of echolocation, previous research conducted under controlled experimental conditions has shown that echolocation improves blind people's spatial sensing ability. The current study investigated if there is also evidence for functional benefits of echolocation in real life. To address this question the current study conducted an online survey. Thirty-seven blind people participated. Linear regression analyses of survey data revealed that, while statistically controlling for participants' gender, age, level of visual function, general health, employment status, level of education, Braille skill, and use of other mobility means, people who use echolocation have higher salary, and higher mobility in unfamiliar places, than people who do not use echolocation. The majority of our participants (34 out of 37) use the long cane, and all participants who reported to echolocate, also reported to use the long cane. This suggests that the benefit of echolocation that we found might be conditional upon the long cane being used as well. The investigation was correlational in nature, and thus cannot be used to determine causality. In addition, the sample was small (N = 37), and one should be cautious when generalizing the current results to the population. The data, however, are consistent with the idea that echolocation offers real-life advantages for blind people, and that echolocation may be involved in peoples' successful adaptation to vision loss.

Keywords: adaptation; blindness; correlation; mobility; regression; vision loss.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Summary of Results. Bars indicate non-parametric measures of effect-size, i.e., Probability of Superiority as suggested by Grissom and Kim (2012), for those predictors for which both linear regression coefficients and non-parametric tests were significant. Predictors are listed separately for variables “Salary,” “Mobility in Familiar Places,” “Mobility in Unfamiliar Places,” and “Relationship Status.” For the variable “Relationship Status” no predictor contributed significantly. For the other variables names of significant predictors are inscribed within each bar. Probability of Superiority estimates the probability that a score randomly drawn from one population will be greater than a score randomly drawn from another population. For example, Probability of Superiority of 0.78 for predictor “Echolocation” for variable “Salary” means that the probability that a randomly drawn salary score from an echolocating population will be greater than a randomly drawn salary score from a non-echolocating population is 0.78.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ammons C. H., Worchel P., Dallenbach K. M. (1953). “Facial vision”: the perception of obstacles out of doors by blindfolded and blindfolded-deafened subjects. Am. J. Psychol. 66, 519–553 - PubMed
    1. Anderson A. J., Vingrys A. J. (2001). Small samples: does size matter? Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 42, 1411–1413 - PubMed
    1. Bowling A. (2005). Just one question: if one question works, why ask several? J. Epidemiol. Community Health, 59, 342–345 10.1136/jech.2004.021204 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Brabyn J. A. (1982). New developments in mobility and orientation aids for the blind. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 29, 285–289 10.1109/TBME.1982.324945 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brown B., Brabyn J. A. (1987). Mobility and low vision: a review. Clin. Exp. Optom. 70, 96–101